Skip to main content
Full access
Regular Article
Published Online: 1 February 2003

Mild Procedural Learning Disturbances in Neuroleptic-Naive Patients With Schizophrenia

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Abstract

This study investigated whether drug-naive patients with schizophrenia present procedural learning disturbances, given that this function is known to involve the striatum and that structural abnormalities of the striatum have been described in these patients. Eleven neuroleptic-free schizophrenic patients were compared with 11 healthy control subjects on a visuomotor procedural learning task (the Mirror Drawing Test). Average performance and learning rate did not differ between the two groups. However, schizophrenia patients were lower in learning smoothness (the consistency with which performance improves from trial to trial), which would indicate difficulty in benefiting from past experience to perform better on a current task. Because low smoothness has recently been found to be associated with striatal dysfunction, these results might indicate a premature dysfunction of the striatum in young drug-naive patients with schizophrenia. However, this dysfunction is not severe enough to prevent learning.
It is now well established that cognitive impairment is a primary feature of schizophrenia and that this may be observed even in patients not treated with neuroleptic medication.14 Dysfunctions in the areas of attention, memory, language, and executive functions have been described in neuroleptic-free patients. However, no study to date has specifically examined procedural learning, a component of implicit long-term memory.
Procedural learning (“learning by doing”) may be defined as the development of skills in which the strategy of execution cannot be explicitly described. Cognitive or motor procedural skills are learned progressively with practice until there is an automatization of the optimal performance. This function is usually assessed with tasks that require unfamiliar motor or cognitive skills. For instance, subjects may be required to learn and replicate an arbitrarily defined visuospatial sequence or to learn to track a moving target along a predictable path. These types of learning were first reported to be impaired in neurological illnesses involving the striatum, such as Huntington's and Parkinson's diseases.57 The striatum was then suggested to be a critical structure involved in procedural learning. In patients with schizophrenia who do not receive neuroleptic treatments, some MRI studies have shown a reduction in the volume of the putamen.8,9 In addition, extrapyramidal symptoms1013 have been described in schizophrenic patients who have never received neuroleptic medication, suggesting a striatal dysfunction.
Given that procedural learning is known to involve the basal ganglia, and that these structures have been found to be abnormal in first-episode patients with schizophrenia,14,15 one would predict that procedural learning disturbances could occur in these patients. To address this question, we chose to assess the performance of a population of patients with schizophrenia on a procedural task, comparing it with that of healthy control subjects. The goal of this research was to pin down the procedural learning deficits that are due to the disease, free of the potential influence of medication. Drug-naive patients have seldom been tested in the past because of the constraints posed by their condition (e.g., acute illness and necessity of treatment).

METHODS

Subjects

Eleven first-episode drug-naive patients with schizophrenia (8 males, 3 females) were enrolled at the hospital psychiatric emergency and outpatient clinic. They were tested within 1 week of their first visit. All of them were psychotic patients who never received neuroleptic treatment. Exclusion criteria were substance abuse, anesthesia or ECT in the last year, neurological disorder, mental retardation, and any other psychiatric diagnosis. Subjects had to have perfect vision. The diagnosis of schizophrenia was made according to DSM-IV criteria and confirmed by two psychiatrists. Among the included patients, 6 had a prior history of psychiatric illness and 5 presented their first psychotic episode. The diagnosis was reconfirmed 6 months after the current assessment. This procedure has already been used and described by the same team1 and is similar to those employed in several studies with the same experimental population.2,8,9,1113
Assessment of clinical symptoms was based on semistructured interviews conducted by trained psychiatrists. The mental status of the patients was scored on the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS).16 Extrapyramidal symptoms were assessed with the Extrapyramidal Symptoms Rating Scale.17 Psychopathology ratings were done within 1 week of the cognitive evaluation. The psychiatrists who did the psychopathological evaluations were blind to the results of the neuropsychological testing. A clinical evaluation took about 1 hour. Patients were tested on the procedural task a few days before beginning their medication.
Eleven healthy control subjects (9 males, 2 females) were also enrolled as a control group. The two groups were similarly distributed for age and education. All participants were right-handed. All control subjects were required to be free of neurological or psychiatric conditions. None of the subjects had any diagnosable neurological disease or suffered from substance abuse other than nicotine dependence. Participants were given a complete description of the study, and their written informed consent was obtained.

Procedural Task

The Mirror Drawing Task requires subjects to learn how to move their hands in a new visuospatial context. This task has been frequently used to assess procedural memory in various clinical populations.1821 Subjects must draw the outline of a picture (a star) while looking only at its reflection in a mirror, so that proprioceptive information from the hand remains normal but visual information is different from what is usually perceived in everyday life. The task includes a baseline measurement followed by 10 learning trials and a final test measurement. The first five trials (first block of trials) and the second five trials (second block of trials) were separated by a 30-minute pause interval in order to verify if there is a persistence of the optimal performance over time. There was also a 30-minute pause between the second block of trials and the final test measurement (last trial).
For each trial, scores included completion time (in seconds) and number of errors (oversteps beyond the outline of the star to be drawn). In order to avoid a speed/accuracy tradeoff (fast performance with numerous errors in some trials and slow performance with few errors in others), the statistical analyses were run on scores computed according to the following equation: [(completion time)×(number of errors+1)]. To control for basic manual dexterity, each participant was required to draw the same picture (the star) without a mirror before the first trial of the task.

Statistical Analyses

Sociodemographic and clinical measures were compared between groups by using one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs). For the Mirror Drawing Task, raw data were normalized into their natural logarithm. Three variables were then created and calculated separately for two blocks of trials. The first variable was Average Performance, the mean score for all trials within each block. The second variable was Learning Rate, expressed by the slope of the trial scores within each block. The steeper the slope (values ranging from 0 to 1), the faster the learning rate. The third variable was Learning Smoothness as estimated by the statistical coefficient of determination (r2) for the trials within a block. The greater the learning smoothness, the better the learning consistency; that is, the progressive improvement from trial to trial. Large intertrial fluctuations were therefore reflected by low learning smoothness.
In addition to the three preceding variables, a score for Drawing Without a Mirror was computed [(completion time)×(number of errors+1)] as a measure of basic manual dexterity. Finally, a score for Oscillations, previously described in patients with frontal lobe lesions1819 and occurring in our patients, was also compiled at each trial. Oscillations were defined as anarchic, “zigzag,” choppy movements occurring during a trial and corresponding to a temporary inability to move in the correct direction. The number of oscillations in each trial was counted by two independent double-blind scorers. In the event of disagreement between scorers for a given trial, the mean of the two scores was used. Given the distribution of the raw scores, the scores were transformed by using their square roots.
To verify the presence of learning effects in the Mirror Drawing Task, t-tests were performed separately for each group and for each block of trials to see if the slopes differed from zero. Then Average Performance, Learning Rate, Learning Smoothness, Drawing Without a Mirror, and Oscillations were compared between groups by use of one-way ANOVAs.

RESULTS

Sociodemographic and Clinical Variables

Table 1 shows the sociodemographic and clinical features for each group. The PANSS total score was 89.3 (SD=7.1). This level of scoring with PANSS is common in studies conducted in unmedicated schizophrenia patients.1,2,811 There was no difference in age (F=0.88, df=1,20, P=0.36) or education (F=0.20, df=1,20, P=0.66) between control subjects and patients.

Mirror Drawing Task

Single-sample t-tests performed separately for each group showed that both groups had learning slopes significantly different from zero (t>3.74, df=10, P<0.01) in the first block of trials. However, in the second block of trials, while control subjects showed significant learning (t=5.31, df=10, P<0.001), the schizophrenia patients did not demonstrate a slope different from zero, although the P-value was near the significant level (t=2.14, df=10, P=0.06).
A one-way ANOVA comparing the two groups on every variable of the task did not reveal significant differences in the score for Drawing Without a Mirror (F=1.00, df=1,20, P=0.33). Figure 1 shows Average Performance of the two groups in each block of trials. Average Performance did not differ significantly on either the first block of trials (F=0.469, df=1,20, P=0.50) or the second (F=1.29, df=1,20, P=0.27). Learning Rate did not differ significantly between the two groups on either the first block (F=2.76, df=1,20, P=0.11) or the second (F=1.73, df=1,20, P=0.20). Learning Smoothness did not differ between groups for the first block of trials (F=0.59, df=1,20, P=0.45), but did differ for the second block (F=4.88, df=1,20, P=0.04). Given that scores were not normally distributed in patients with schizophrenia, a nonparametric test (Wilcoxon Mann-Whitney) was performed for this variable. Like the parametric analysis, this test also yielded a significant difference (P=0.02). Finally, an ANOVA performed on the Oscillation scores revealed no between-group difference (F=1.00, df=1,20, P=0.33).

DISCUSSION

To our knowledge, this is the first time an assessment of procedural learning has been done in first-episode drug-naive patients with schizophrenia. The present study revealed that neuroleptic-naive schizophrenia patients are capable of learning a visuomotor procedural task, despite some degree of abnormality. Learning slopes and general level of performance were comparable to those of the normal control subjects. However, from a qualitative point of view, the learning profile of the patients with schizophrenia was slightly different. The patients were found to have a lower learning smoothness than control subjects; that is, there were more intertrial fluctuations in performance. This difference could not be explained by age, education, or basic manual dexterity, since these control variables were not different between groups.
Considering that the patients in this study were impaired not so much in the degree, but in the quality of procedural learning, one may ask how significant such intertrial fluctuations are from a clinical perspective. Patients with schizophrenia do learn tasks, but they do not benefit as much as normal subjects from the immediately preceding trial. In other words, their learning is less efficient (two steps forward and one step back) than that of normal control subjects (who start each new trial at a level better than the preceding one). From a practical viewpoint, one might visualize learning smoothness in terms of the example of learning to ride a bicycle. After some trials, performances would certainly improve in both normal subjects and patients with schizophrenia. However, while normal subjects would progressively improve their riding from each trial to the following one, schizophrenia patients might occasionally fall and hurt themselves. They both can learn within the same period of time, with the same final performance, but their learning smoothness is not the same. In other words, the harmony of learning is affected in schizophrenia patients when compared with normal subjects.
Procedural learning is crucial to many aspects of daily living, and it seems to have a significant influence on educational and occupational rehabilitation of patients with schizophrenia. The rudimentary employment available to the patients often involves repetitive motor and cognitive sequences that must be progressively developed. A procedural learning disturbance such as the one described in the current study may therefore have some effect in clinical practice or in rehabilitation programs.
Learning smoothness disturbances have also been described in visuomotor procedural learning tasks performed by schizophrenia patients chronically treated with neuroleptics.21,22 In these patients, the severity of the learning smoothness deficit was found to correlate with the striatal dopamine D2 receptor occupancy, as measured in vivo with 123I-iodobenzamide.22 However, the poor learning smoothness described in the present study cannot be attributed to an elevated striatal D2 receptor occupancy, since these patients have never been treated with neuroleptics. It is therefore possible that an intrinsic striatal dysfunction occurs in these drug-naive schizophrenia patients. This possibility is supported by imaging studies showing a caudate atrophy in these patients.9,15 In addition, extrapyramidal symptoms, which are usually associated with dysfunctions within the basal ganglia, have been described in such drug-naive patients,1013 although these motor dysfunctions were not observed in the patients of the present study.
The lack of intergroup difference in the number of oscillations suggests that the patients' frontal capacity (executive functions) to learn new motor skills remains intact.18,19 This would not be in accordance with the literature showing that patients with schizophrenia have executive dysfunctions associated with functional or structural impairments of the frontal cortex.2325 However, although such executive and frontal dysfunctions have been described in both drug-naive and chronically treated schizophrenia patients, the latter are usually more affected than the former. This difference has been addressed by Gallhofer et al.,26 who demonstrated that the severity of the executive deficits in schizophrenia increases with the duration of the illness. Oscillations in the Mirror Drawing Task would therefore not be sensitive enough to permit detection of executive deficits in the young drug-naive patients included in the present study. However, in another study performed on chronic institutionalized patients with schizophrenia,21 this measure was found useful in detecting executive malfunctioning.
Limitations on the interpretation of the findings can be noted. Procedural learning appears as a heterogeneous function that is highly dependent on the sensorial, motor, or intellectual modality involved in the task. It is therefore possible that our results apply only to motor procedural learning. Further studies are needed with other procedural learning tasks involving other modalities.
In summary, procedural learning smoothness may be affected in young drug-naive schizophrenia patients, and this impairment appears to have an early onset, given that it can be measured well before antipsychotic treatment is initiated. In other words, the illness itself seems to account for this feature. Although this learning disturbance does not prevent patients from learning procedures, it may suggest a striatal dysfunction. Our findings concur with other evidence, such as the occurrence of extrapyramidal symptoms and the caudate/ putamen volume abnormalities, that suggests young neuroleptic-naive schizophrenia patients may have a striatal dysfunction.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank Mrs. J. Lemaire and J. Bégin for their help with the statistical analyses; Dr. M. Sasseville for assisting with enrollment of patients; and Dr. F. Richer for his helpful comments about the Mirror Drawing Task. This research was supported by Fonds de la Recherche en Santé du Québec (FRSQ).
FIGURE 1. Average Performance of the two groups (neuroleptic-naive schizophrenia patients, n=11; control subjects, n=11) in each block of five trials. ln=natural logarithm. *One-way analysis of variance, P<0.05.
TABLE 1. Sociodemographic and clinical features of each group

References

1.
Lussier I, Stip E: Memory and attention deficits in drug-naive patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2001; 48:45-55
2.
Saykin AJ, Shtasel DL, Gur RE, et al: Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic-naive patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:124-131
3.
Heinrichs RW, Zakzanis K: Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 1999; 12:426-445
4.
Mohamed S, Paulsen JS, O'Leary D, et al: Generalized cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:749-754
5.
Harrington DL, Haaland KY, Yeo RA, et al: Procedural memory in Parkinson's disease: impaired motor but not visuoperceptual learning. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1990; 12:323-339
6.
Knopman D, Nissen MJ: Procedural learning is impaired in Huntington's disease: evidence from the serial reaction time task. Neuropsychologia 1991; 29:245-254
7.
Saint-Cyr JA, Taylor AE, Lang AE: Procedural learning and neostriatal dysfunction in man. Brain 1988; 111:941-959
8.
Lang DJ, Kopala LC, Smith GN, et al: MRI study of basal ganglia volumes in drug-naive first-episode patients with schizophrenia (abstract). Schizophr Res 1999; 36:202
9.
Gur RE, Maany V, Mozley PD, et al: Subcortical MRI volumes in neuroleptic-naive and treated patients with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1998; 155:1711-1717
10.
Caligiuri MP, Lohr JB: Instrumental motor predictors of neuroleptic-induced parkinsonism in newly medicated schizophrenia patients. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1997; 9:562-567
11.
Cortese L: Movement disorders and clinical correlates in drug-naive first episode patients with psychosis (abstract). Mov Disord 2001; 16(suppl 1):S51
12.
Puri BK, Barnes TR, Chapman MJ, et al: Spontaneous dyskinesia in first episode schizophrenia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999; 66:76-80
13.
Chatterjee A, Chakos M, Koreen A, et al: Prevalence and clinical correlates of extrapyramidal signs and spontaneous dyskinesia in never-medicated schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry 1995; 152:1724-1729
14.
Corson PW, Nopoulos P, Andreasen NC, et al: Caudate size in first-episode neuroleptic-naive schizophrenic patients measured using an artificial neural network. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 46:712-720
15.
Lang DJ, Kopala L, Vandorpe RA, et al: Basal ganglia volumes and treatment-response in never-medicated, first episode schizophrenia patients (abstract). Schizophr Res 2001; 49:159
16.
Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler, LA: The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13:261-276
17.
Chouinard G, Ross-Chouinard A, Annable L, et al: Extrapyramidal Symptoms Rating Scale (abstract). Can J Neurol Sci 1980; 7:233
18.
Chouinard MJ, Rouleau I, Richer F: Closed-loop sensorimotor control and acquisition after frontal lesions. Brain Cogn 1998; 37:178-182
19.
Richer F, Chouinard MJ, Rouleau I: Frontal lesions impair the attentional control during motor learning. Neuropsychologia 1998; 37:1427-1435
20.
Bhushan B, Dwivedi CB, Mishra R, et al: Performance on a mirror-drawing task by non-right-handers. J Gen Psychol 2000; 127:271-277
21.
Bedard MA, Scherer H, Stip E, et al: Procedural learning in schizophrenia: further consideration on the deleterious effect of neuroleptics. Brain Cogn 2000; 43:31-39
22.
Soucy JP, Bedard MA, Stip E, et al: Relationship between procedural learning and striatal D2 receptor binding in schizophrenic patients treated with olanzapine (abstract). Soc Neurosci 1999; 25:2074
23.
Andreasen NC, Rezai K, Alliger R, et al: Hypofrontality in neuroleptic-naive patients and in patients with chronic schizophrenia. assessment with xenon 133 single-photon emission computed tomography and the Tower of London. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992; 49:943-958
24.
Rubin P, Holm S, Friberg L, et al: Altered modulation of prefrontal and subcortical brain activity in newly diagnosed schizophrenia and schizophreniform disorder: a regional cerebral blood flow study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48:987-995
25.
Goldberg TE, Saint-Cyr JA, Weinberger DR: Assessment of procedural learning and problem solving in schizophrenic patients by Tower of Hanoi-type tasks. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 1990; 2:165-173
26.
Gallhofer B, Bauer U, Lis S, et al: Cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia: comparison of treatment with atypical antipsychotic agents and conventional neuroleptic drugs. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 1996; 6(suppl 2):S13-S20

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 58 - 63
PubMed: 12556572

History

Published online: 1 February 2003
Published in print: February 2003

Authors

Details

Hélène Schérer, Ph.D.
Received March 20, 2001; revised November 1, 2001; accepted November 14, 2001. From the Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Université du Québec à Montréal (UQAM) (h.s., f.p., m.b.) and Fernand-Séguin Research Center, (h.s., e.s., m.b.) Montréal, Canada; and Université de Montréal, Québec, Canada (e.s.). Address correspondence to Dr. Bédard, Cognitive Neuroscience Center, UQAM, P.O. Box 8888, Montreal, Quebec, H3C 3P8, Canada. E-mail:[email protected]
Emmanuel Stip, M.D., M.Sc.
Received March 20, 2001; revised November 1, 2001; accepted November 14, 2001. From the Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Université du Québec à Montréal (UQAM) (h.s., f.p., m.b.) and Fernand-Séguin Research Center, (h.s., e.s., m.b.) Montréal, Canada; and Université de Montréal, Québec, Canada (e.s.). Address correspondence to Dr. Bédard, Cognitive Neuroscience Center, UQAM, P.O. Box 8888, Montreal, Quebec, H3C 3P8, Canada. E-mail:[email protected]
François Paquet, B.Sc.
Received March 20, 2001; revised November 1, 2001; accepted November 14, 2001. From the Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Université du Québec à Montréal (UQAM) (h.s., f.p., m.b.) and Fernand-Séguin Research Center, (h.s., e.s., m.b.) Montréal, Canada; and Université de Montréal, Québec, Canada (e.s.). Address correspondence to Dr. Bédard, Cognitive Neuroscience Center, UQAM, P.O. Box 8888, Montreal, Quebec, H3C 3P8, Canada. E-mail:[email protected]
Marc-André Bédard, Ph.D.
Received March 20, 2001; revised November 1, 2001; accepted November 14, 2001. From the Cognitive Neuroscience Center, Université du Québec à Montréal (UQAM) (h.s., f.p., m.b.) and Fernand-Séguin Research Center, (h.s., e.s., m.b.) Montréal, Canada; and Université de Montréal, Québec, Canada (e.s.). Address correspondence to Dr. Bédard, Cognitive Neuroscience Center, UQAM, P.O. Box 8888, Montreal, Quebec, H3C 3P8, Canada. E-mail:[email protected]

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share