Skip to main content
Full access
REGULAR
Published Online: 1 July 2010

Nonclinical Obsessive-Compulsive Symptoms and Executive Functions in Schizophrenia

Publication: The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
A growing literature suggests that obsessive-compulsive symptoms (OCS) and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) occur in a higher proportion of patients with schizophrenia than was originally suspected. Clinical and epidemiological studies suggest that more than one-third of individuals with schizophrenia display OCS, and prevalence rates for OCD have ranged between 7.8% and 40.5%. 13 Additionally, an elevated rate of OCD has been found in first episode and drug naive patients with schizophrenia, 1 indicating that it is not accounted for by the course of the primary illness or antipsychotic medication alone. The presence of OCD or significant OCS in patients with schizophrenia has been associated with more severe psychosis 4 and depression, 4 poorer social functioning, 2 lesser likelihood of being employed, 5 and poorer prognosis. 5
It has been suggested that comorbid OCS or OCD in schizophrenia may represent a specific pattern of neurobiological dysfunction. 6, 7 A number of studies have evaluated the relationship between neuropsychological functioning and OCS in schizophrenia, but the findings have been inconsistent. One study found a correlation between higher levels of OCS and poorer delayed visual memory, as well as decreased cognitive flexibility as measured by the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and the Trail Making test. 8 Others have also observed an association between OCS and poorer executive functioning as measured by the WCST, 9, 10 though failures to find a relationship with either the WCST or other tests of executive function have also been reported. 1113
In patients with OCD who do not have schizophrenia, symptom subtypes such as hoarding, checking and washing have been found to be differentially associated with functional neuroimaging measures. 14, 15 In one factor analytic study of neuropsychological functioning, three major subsets of patients were observed with differential patterns of association between OCS subtypes and performance on the WCST and part B of the Trail Making test. 16 One subset of patients was impaired on both tests and had prominent contamination obsessions and compulsive washing. A second subset was intact on both tests and was characterized by aggressive obsessions. A third subset, consisting of a mixed group of patients with either prominent obsessions and aggression or prominent washing compulsions, was slow to complete the Trail Making test, with intact WCST performance. Another study found that patients with schizophrenia and comorbid OCD characterized by prominent checking symptoms showed poorer performance on tests of response inhibition and cognitive flexibility when compared with patients with primarily washing symptoms. 17 A relationship between more impaired executive function and checking has also been noted in studies of nonclinical samples. 18, 19 These findings suggest that the presence of different OCS subtypes may be contributing to the heterogeneity of relationships observed between OCS and neuropsychological functioning in patients with schizophrenia.
The current study evaluated the hypothesis that greater overall severity of OCS in patients with schizophrenia, who do not meet clinical criteria for OCD, would be associated with more impaired executive functioning as evaluated using questionnaire and performance-based measures. Based on the work in OCD, we also hypothesized that among the OCS subtypes, more severe obsessing, washing, and checking will be associated with poorer executive functioning.

METHODS

Participants

Participants included 29 patients with a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia with no history of OCD, confirmed using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR (SCID I/P), 20 as well as 32 healthy comparison subjects. Groups were matched on gender and parental education. Patients were recruited from local outpatient clinics and by referrals from treating physicians. Healthy comparison participants were recruited through advertisements in local newspapers and the use of fliers and did not meet criteria for any past or current psychiatric illness as confirmed with the SCID I/NP. Participants were excluded if they had a history of neurological disorder (e.g., epilepsy), mental retardation, traumatic brain injury, or significant systemic medical illness. Participants were also excluded if they met criteria for a substance use disorder (other than nicotine) in the 6 months prior to participation. All patients with schizophrenia were receiving stable doses of antipsychotic medications at the time of the study. After complete description of the study, all participants gave written informed consent following a protocol approved by the Dartmouth College Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects and received financial compensation for their participation.

Measures

Psychiatric Symptoms

The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory—Revised (OCI-R) 21 is an 18-item questionnaire, rated on a 5-point scale, used to evaluate the overall severity of OCS as well as the severity of six symptom subtypes including checking, washing, hoarding, ordering, obsessing, and neutralizing. This six subtype structure was identified via factor analysis and has been empirically validated in clinical and nonclinical samples. 22, 23 Clinical symptoms were assessed using the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms 24 and Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms. 25 Overall severity of psychopathology was evaluated with the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. 26 The Beck Depression Inventory—II (BDI-II) 27 was used to assess current symptoms of depression.

Executive Functions

Participants completed the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Adult Version (BRIEF-A) self-report form. 28 The BRIEF-A is a 75-item questionnaire designed to assess executive functioning in daily life over the prior month, with higher scores reflecting poorer executive functioning. Subscales of the BRIEF-A include inhibit, shift, emotional control, self-monitor, initiate, working memory, plan/organize, task monitor, and organization of materials. Informant data on the BRIEF-A was also available for a subset of 15 patients and 30 comparison subjects.
Performance-based executive function measures included the WCST 29 and the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Trail Making subtest. 30 In the WCST, participants are asked to match a series of response cards that have various forms, which vary in color and number, to one of four static stimulus cards. The sorting principle (i.e., color, form, or number) must be deduced by the participant based on feedback provided by the examiner after each response, with changes in the sorting principle occurring without warning. The number of categories completed and perseverative errors, as well as failures to maintain set, are reported here. The WCST assesses abilities such as cognitive flexibility and problem solving in response to changing environmental contingencies. The Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Trail Making subtest involves five timed trials, the first requiring scanning letters and numbers on a sheet of paper and marking off all the number threes, the second, connecting numbers in ascending order, the third, connecting letters in alphabetical order, the fourth, switching between connecting letters and numbers in their respective orders, and the fifth, a trial involving drawing a line over a dotted line. The test assesses several cognitive functions including visual scanning and psychomotor speed, with the fourth trial placing considerable demand on cognitive flexibility. The total time to complete each trial is reported, with longer time indicating worse performance. Baseline intellectual functioning was estimated using the scaled score from the Wide Range Achievement Test—3rd edition (WRAT-3) reading subtest. 31 In this test, participants are required to read aloud words printed on a card, with higher scores indicating better word recognition. The total raw score for WRAT-3 reading is converted to a standard score with a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15.

Data Analysis

Participant characteristic data were analyzed using parametric and nonparametric statistics, as appropriate. Group differences in clinical and neuropsychological characteristics were assessed using multivariate analyses of covariance (MANCOVAs), controlling for age, reading score, and depression score. Significance level was set to p<0.05. In the patient sample, the relationships between OCI-R total and subscale scores and both self-report and performance-based measures of executive functioning were evaluated using partial correlations controlling for age, reading score, and depression score. As the BRIEF-A scales had distributions that deviated significantly from normality, we applied a Box-Cox transformation to the mean scores for all scales prior to statistical analysis.

RESULTS

Table 1 presents the participant characteristic and clinical data. The groups were well matched for sex composition, handedness, and parental education. The patient group was significantly older (p=0.006), was more depressed (p=0.001), and had a lower reading standard score (p=0.01) than the healthy comparison group. These latter variables were therefore statistically controlled for in subsequent analyses. The schizophrenia group obtained a significantly higher score than the comparison group on the obsessing subscale of the OCI-R (p=0.02), despite controlling for age, reading and depression. Groups were equivalent on the other subscales and total OCI-R score. No significant correlations were observed between any of the OCI-R scores and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms, Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms, or the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale.
TABLE 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics
Table 2 presents the performance-based and the self- and informant-report executive function scores. Patients with schizophrenia performed significantly slower than the comparison group on all conditions of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Trail Making subtest and completed fewer categories on the WCST. The patients reported having significantly greater problems with their daily executive functioning abilities on the BRIEF-A, including higher mean scores on the shift (p=0.03) and working memory (p=0.004) scales, with a trend noted for emotional control (p=0.09). Informant data showed significantly greater problems with the patients’ daily executive abilities for all BRIEF-A scales with the exception of emotional control, which was at the level of a trend (p=0.05).
TABLE 2. Group Differences on Executive Function Measures Controlling for Age, Reading Score, and Depression Score
Table 3 presents the correlations between OCS severity and executive functioning measures for the schizophrenia group. With respect to self-reported executive functions, greater overall OCS severity was only significantly correlated with poorer scores on the task monitor scale. Among the OCS subtypes, only obsessing, hoarding, and washing severity were associated with self-reported problems in aspects of executive functions. Obsessing was associated with worse scores on the shift, emotional control, initiate, plan/organize, working memory, and task monitoring scales. More severe hoarding was related to poorer task monitoring and organization of materials. Washing was correlated with a better (i.e., less impaired) working memory score. As with the self-report, more severe obsessing was related to worse BRIEF-A informant-report shift, self-monitor, initiate, plan/organize, and task monitor scores. More severe hoarding was associated with poorer shift, self-monitor, and task monitor scores. Checking was correlated with worse shift, initiate, and task monitor scores.
TABLE 3. Partial Correlations Between OCI-R Scores and Neuropsychological Variables in the Schizophrenia Group Controlling for Age, Reading Score, and Depression Score
The correlations between OCI-R responses and performance on executive functioning measures also differed depending on the OCS subtype. Greater overall OCS severity was not associated with neuropsychological performance on any of the administered tasks. Checking, hoarding and neutralizing symptoms were associated with aspects of Trail Making test performance, though only hoarding was associated with poorer switching ability. Performance on the WCST was not significantly correlated with any of the OCI-R scores.

DISCUSSION

Our group of patients with schizophrenia reported greater difficulty, relative to the healthy comparison group, with certain aspects of their executive functions in daily life including their ability to adjust to changes in routine and use their working memory. Additionally, the patients’ informants observed more difficulty with nearly all aspects of executive functions in daily life. The patient group also obtained lower scores than the comparison group on two performance-based measures of executive function, Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Trail Making subtest and the WCST. These findings are consistent with the extant literature indicating the presence of executive dysfunction in patients with schizophrenia. 32
With respect to OCS, the schizophrenia group was found to have higher scores than the comparison group only on the obsessing subscale of the OCI-R. This is inconsistent with previous literature suggesting that patients with schizophrenia experience multiple types of OCS. 33 In looking at the items that assess obsessive symptomatology on the OCI-R, it is worth noting that there is no overlap with symptoms that are specifically related to psychosis. Furthermore, we did not find a correlation between obsessing or any other OCS and the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. The presence of greater obsessing in the schizophrenia group thus cannot be accounted for by severity of the primary illness. Another possibility is that our comparison group’s OCS scores are higher than may be expected for a healthy adult sample, thereby reducing the likelihood of finding a significant difference between this group and the patient sample. The mean OCI-R score in our healthy group, however, is within a standard deviation of scores obtained in several other studies of the instrument in healthy adult samples, 21, 34, 35 arguing against this possibility. Further research is required to determine whether our finding is specific to the patient sample in the present investigation and, if so, what factors may be contributing to the heterogeneity of obsessive-compulsive symptom subtype presentation in different samples of patients with schizophrenia.
Overall OCS severity in the patients with schizophrenia was associated with poorer self- and informant-reported ability to monitor task performance for accuracy, as well as poorer cognitive flexibility per informant report. The former finding may be related to evidence from electrophysiological studies showing exaggerated performance monitoring in adults with primary OCD. 36 This may reflect the doubt that many of these patients have with respect to their compulsive behaviors, and/or reduced confidence in their cognitive abilities, particularly with respect to attention. 37 That overall OCS severity was correlated with cognitive flexibility is consistent with studies implicating this executive function in patients with primary OCD. 38
Various subtypes of OCS have been reported to be differentially related to the integrity of neural circuitry and cognitive functioning in patients with primary OCD. In the present investigation, we observed that the severity of nonclinical OCS subtypes was also differentially related to executive functions in patients with schizophrenia who have never met DSM-IV criteria for OCD. Obsessing symptoms were associated with decreased ability to monitor task performance for accuracy, to initiate activities, to plan/organize tasks, and to think flexibly, as reported by both the patients and their informants. In addition, more severe obsessing was related to greater difficulty with emotional control and working memory per self-report and worse ability to monitor the effects of one’s behavior on others per informant report. That obsessing was associated with several executive functions may be especially noteworthy given that only this symptom subtype was reported as being more severe by the patient relative to the comparison group. Patients with primary OCD have also been reported to exhibit problems in cognitive flexibility and several other aspects of executive functions. 39, 40 We speculate that the nature of obsessing symptoms, being recurrent and requiring significant mental energy, might render it more difficult for an individual to allocate attentional resources to task-relevant cognitive processes such as monitoring, planning, working memory, and cognitive flexibility.
More severe hoarding behavior was related to poorer organization of materials needed for tasks per self-report. This appears consistent with the nature of hoarding, which involves the acquisition of, and failure to discard, a large number of generally useless possessions, causing significant clutter and interference with daily functioning. Hoarding was also found to be associated with more difficulty with task monitoring per self and informant report. This finding may be related to evidence that patients with compulsive hoarding have slower reaction times and difficulty distinguishing target and nontarget stimuli on performance-based neuropsychological measures. 41 Finally, hoarding was related to worse informant report cognitive flexibility and monitoring of the effects of one’s behavior on others.
Prior investigations have reported an association between compulsive checking and poorer scores on performance-based tests of executive function in nonclinical samples 18, 19 and in patients with schizophrenia and comorbid OCD. 17 In our schizophrenia group we observed a relationship between more severe nonclinical compulsive checking and worse executive functions in daily life per informant report, specifically for cognitive flexibility, initiation, and task monitoring. Checking was unrelated, however, to performance-based measures of executive functions. Indeed, in contrast to the self and informant report measures, few correlations were found between OCS subtypes and executive function as assessed via performance-based tests. Hoarding was associated with decreased cognitive flexibility on Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Trail Making subtest but not on the WCST. This is consistent with a study of patients with primary OCD which found that hoarding was unrelated to WCST performance. 38 The lack of relationship between WCST performance and severity of nonclinical OCS is consistent with several studies that have failed to observe a relationship between the WCST and OCS in schizophrenia. 1113
Overall, our findings are generally consistent with previous work indicating that OCS in patients with schizophrenia may represent a specific pattern of neurobiological dysfunction. 6, 7 Furthermore, the results indicate that nonclinical OCS can contribute to the heterogeneity of cognitive functioning commonly seen in studies of schizophrenia. It is important, however, to note limitations of this study. The possibility of a type I error is increased given the multiple statistical comparisons conducted. In addition, the correlational nature of the study precludes determination of a direction of causality, that is, whether more severe nonclinical OCS contributes to greater executive dysfunction or vice versa. We hypothesize that the considerable evidence indicating the presence of executive dysfunction in primary OCD suggests that the latter relationship is more likely. An additional limitation is that the modest sample size may limit generalization of the results. Replication in a larger sample of patients with schizophrenia and nonclinical OCS will be important. Finally, determination of the specificity and generalizability of the pattern of nonclinical OCS and their association with executive functions in schizophrenia would benefit from direct comparison to patients with primary OCD and to healthy adults with high levels of nonclinical obsessive-compulsive symptoms.
In summary, the present findings indicate that the integrity of executive functions in schizophrenia may depend in part on the subtype of OCS exhibited, even when these symptoms are at a nonclinical level. Furthermore, the presence of specific OCS subtypes may have implications for treatment planning. Certain OCS subtypes, such as hoarding, have been associated with poorer responsiveness to standard pharmacological and behavioral treatments in patients with primary OCD. 42 It is possible that OCS subtype also contributes to the responsiveness of OCS to treatment in schizophrenia, raising the need to consider alternative treatment strategies. Additionally, evidence of a contribution of executive functions to OCS in schizophrenia suggests that the application of cognitive remediation strategies may not only help such patients with executive dysfunction 43 but perhaps also ameliorate their obsessive-compulsive symptoms.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by grants from the Hitchcock Foundation, the Stanley Medical Research Institute, and the National Alliance for Medical Image Computing Grant U54 EB005149. The authors would like to thank Matthew Garlinghouse, Margaret Grove, and Tor Tosteson for statistical support. Dr. Roth is an author of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function–Adult Version and receives a royalty from the publisher. The authors declare no other conflict of interest related to the content of this article.

Footnote

Received February 2, 2009; revised April 9, 2009; accepted April 17, 2009. The authors are affiliated with the Neuropsychology Program, Department of Psychiatry, at Dartmouth-Hitchcock Medical Center, in Lebanon, New Hampshire. Address correspondence to Robert M. Roth, Ph.D., Dartmouth Medical School, Department of Psychiatry, One Medical Center Dr., Lebanon, NH 03756-0001; [email protected] (e-mail).
Copyright © 2010 American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.

References

1.
Poyurovsky M, Fuchs C, Weizman A: Obsessive-compulsive disorder in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:1998–2000
2.
Poyurovsky M, Hramenkov S, Isakov V, et al: Obsessive-compulsive disorder in hospitalized patients with chronic schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res 2001; 102:49–57
3.
Poyurovsky M, Bergman J, Weizman R: Obsessive-compulsive disorder in elderly schizophrenia patients. J Psychiatr Res 2006; 40:189–191
4.
Kayahan B, Ozturk O, Veznedaroglu B, et al: Obsessive-compulsive symptoms in schizophrenia: prevalance and clinical correlates. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2005; 59:291–295
5.
Berman I, Kalinowski A, Berman SM, et al: Obsessive and compulsive symptoms in chronic schizophrenia. Compr Psychiatry 1995; 36:6–10
6.
Gross-Isseroff R, Hermesh H, Zohar J, et al: Neuroimaging communality between schizophrenia and obsessive compulsive disorder: a putative basis for schizo-obsessive disorder? World J Biol Psychiatry 2003; 4:129–134
7.
Poyurovsky M, Kriss V, Weisman G, et al: Familial aggregation of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders and obsessive-compulsive associated disorders in schizophrenia probands with and without OCD. Am J Med Genet Neuropsychiatr Genet 2005; 133:31–36
8.
Berman I, Merson A, Viegner B, et al: Obsessions and compulsions as a distinct cluster of symptoms in schizophrenia: a neuropsychological study. J Nerv Ment Dis 1998; 186:150–156
9.
Hwang MY, Morgan JE, Losconzcy MF: Clinical and neuropsychological profiles of obsessive-compulsive schizophrenia: a pilot study. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2000; 12:91–94
10.
Lysaker PH, Bryson GJ, Marks KA, et al: Association of obsessions and compulsions in schizophrenia with neurocognition and negative symptoms. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002; 14:449–453
11.
Borkowska A, Pilaczynska E, Rybakowski JK: The frontal lobe neuropsychological tests in patients with schizophrenia and/or obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2003; 15:359–362
12.
Ongur D, Goff DC: Obsessive-compulsive symptoms in schizophrenia: associated clinical features, cognitive function, and medication status. Schizophrenia Res 2004; 75:349–362
13.
Whitney KA, Fastenau PS, Evans JD, et al: Comparative neuropsychological function in obsessive-compulsive disorder and schizophrenia with and without obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Schizophr Res 2004; 69:75–83
14.
Mataix-Cols D, Wooderson S, Lawrence N, et al: Distinct neural correlates of washing, checking, and hoarding symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004; 61:564–576
15.
Saxena S, Brody AL, Maidment KM, et al: Cerebral glucose metabolism in obsessive-compulsive hoarding. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:1038–1048
16.
Duran ET, Garcell JR, Zamorano ER, et al: Neuropsychological characterization in clinical subtypes of an obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) sample of patients. Salud Ment 2007; 30:1–8
17.
Omori IM, Murata Y, Yamanishi T, et al: The differential impact of executive attention dysfunction on episodic memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder patients with checking symptoms vs. those with washing symptoms. J Psychiatr Res 2007; 41:776–784
18.
Mataix-Cols D, Junque C, Sanchez-Turet M, et al: Neuropsychological functioning in a subclinical obsessive-compulsive sample. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:898–904
19.
Roth RM, Baribeau J: Performance of subclinical compulsive checkers on putative tests of frontal and temporal lobe memory functions. J Nerv Ment Dis 1996; 184:411–416
20.
First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, et al: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders (SCID-I/P), Patient Edition. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Publishing, 2002
21.
Foa EB, Huppert JD, Leiberg S, et al: The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: development and validation of a short version. Psychol Assessment 2002; 14:485–496
22.
Fullana MA, Tortella-Feliu M, Caseras X, et al: Psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the obsessive-compulsive inventory-revised in a non-clinical sample. J Anxiety Disord 2005; 19:893–903
23.
Huppert JD, Walther MR, Hajcak G, et al: The OCI-R: validation of the subscales in a clinical sample. J Anxiety Disord 2007; 21:394–406
24.
Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City, College of Medicine, University of Iowa, 1984
25.
Andreasen NC: Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City, College of Medicine, University of Iowa, 1984
26.
Overall JE, Gorham DR: The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychol Reports 1962; 10:799–812
27.
Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK: Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II). San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 1996
28.
Roth RM, Isquith PK, Gioia GA: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Adult Version (BRIEF-A). Lutz, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 2005
29.
Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL, et al: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) Manual Revised and Expanded. Odessa, Fla, Psychological Assessment Resources, 1993
30.
Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH: Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. San Antonio, Tex, Psychological Corp, 2001
31.
Wilkinson G: Wide Range Achievement Test, 3rd ed. Wilmington, Del, Jastek, 1993
32.
Sharma T, Antonova L: Cognitive function in schizophrenia: deficits, functional consequences, and future treatment. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2003; 26:25–40
33.
Poyurovsky M, Weizman A, Weizman R: Obsessive-compulsive disorder in schizophrenia: clinical characteristics and treatment. CNS Drugs 2004; 18:989–1010
34.
Sica C, Ghisi M, Altoè G, et al: The Italian version of the Obsessive Compulsive Inventory: its psychometric properties on community and clinical samples. J Anxiety Disord 2009; 23:204–211
35.
Zermatten A, Van der Linden M, Jermann F, et al: Validation of a French version of the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory—Revised in a non-clinical sample. Revue Européenne de Psychologie Appliquée 2006; 56:151–155
36.
Gehring WJ, Himle J, Nisenson LG: Action-monitoring dysfunction in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychol Sci 2000; 11:1–6
37.
Hermans D, Engelen U, Grouwels L, et al: Cognitive confidence in obsessive-compulsive disorder: distrusting perception, attention and memory. Behav Res Ther 2008; 46:98–113
38.
Lawrence NS, Wooderson S, Mataix-Cols D, et al: Decision making and set shifting impairments are associated with distinct symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology 2006; 20:409–419
39.
Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Menzies LA, et al: Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:335–338
40.
van den Heuvel OA, Veltman DJ, Groenewegen HJ, et al: Frontal-striatal dysfunction during planning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62:301–309
41.
Grisham JR, Brown TA, Savage CR, et al: Neuropsychological impairment associated with compulsive hoarding. Behav Res Ther 2007; 45:1471–1483
42.
Starcevic V, Brakoulias V: Symptom subtypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder: are they relevant for treatment? Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2008; 42:651–661
43.
McGurk SR, Twamley EW, Sitzer DI, et al: A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2007; 164:1791–1802

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Go to The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences
Pages: 304 - 312
PubMed: 20686137

History

Published online: 1 July 2010
Published in print: Summer, 2010

Authors

Details

Sheba R. Kumbhani, Ph.D.
Carrie L. Kruck, M.Psych.
Laura A. Flashman, Ph.D.
Thomas W. McAllister, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

PPV Articles - Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share