Skip to main content
Full access
Article
Published Online: 1 February 2004

Neurobiology of Smooth Pursuit Eye Movement Deficits in Schizophrenia: An fMRI Study

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Smooth pursuit eye movements are abnormal in patients with schizophrenia. The investigators used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to compare brain hemodynamic response during a smooth pursuit eye movement task in patients with schizophrenia and healthy comparison subjects. METHOD: Fourteen patients with schizophrenia and 14 healthy comparison subjects performed a smooth pursuit eye movement task while undergoing 1.5-T fMRI. Echo-planar images from the blocked design were evaluated with both a whole-brain random-effects analysis and a region-of-interest analysis. Smooth pursuit deficits were assessed outside the fMRI apparatus by using infrared oculography and were assessed during scanning by evaluating echo-planar time-series data from the eyes. RESULTS: Compared to the healthy subjects, the patients with schizophrenia exhibited greater activity in both posterior hippocampi and the right fusiform gyrus during smooth pursuit eye movements. The region-of-interest analysis revealed subtle activity deficits in frontal and occipital regions in the patients with schizophrenia. Smooth pursuit deficits in the subjects with schizophrenia included lower gain (eye velocity relative to target velocity) and a higher percentage of total eye movements due to anticipatory saccades, compared with the healthy subjects. CONCLUSIONS: The observed group differences are consistent with evidence for diminished inhibitory function in the hippocampus as well as for a disturbance in a frontotemporal network subserving smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia.
Smooth pursuit eye movement abnormalities are among the most reproducible physiologic abnormalities associated with schizophrenia (1). Tracking of a moving target can be performed by two neuronal systems: 1) a smooth pursuit system that precisely matches the foveal area of the retina to the visual image of the target or 2) saccadic movements that rapidly bring the target image to the fovea. Normal subjects perform smooth pursuit eye movement tasks using their smooth pursuit system almost exclusively. Many investigators have proposed that the fundamental deficit in schizophrenia involves a failure to inhibit the saccade system, resulting in the intrusion of saccades during the smooth pursuit task (24). Small saccades ahead of the target motion that span less than 4° of the visual field (leading saccades) show the most significant differences in schizophrenia (5). Other researchers have emphasized the importance of low pursuit gain (the relationship of the velocity of the eye movement to the velocity of the target) and the concomitant increase in compensatory (catch-up) saccades (68).
Most studies investigating the functional neuroanatomy of smooth pursuit abnormalities have tested hypotheses indirectly by exploring the relationship between pursuit quality and performance on tasks thought to isolate distinct components of the pursuit task. For example, Chen and colleagues (9) reported that patients with schizophrenia have a diminished ability to discriminate the velocity of moving targets, arguing for the involvement of primary motion processing areas of the brain such as the middle temporal visual area (V5) and the middle superior temporal area. Examination of pursuit initiation and maintenance with foveofugal and foveopetal ramp tracking tasks has demonstrated the importance of higher visual centers such as the frontal eye fields and their cortical connections in smooth pursuit abnormalities (8). Pursuit-related frontal eye field dysfunction has also been observed in a PET study examining relatives of schizophrenic patients (10). Altered resting frontostriatal glucose metabolism has also been associated with pursuit abnormalities in schizophrenia (11).
To our knowledge, no study has used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate the neurobiology underlying smooth pursuit abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia. Compared to other brain imaging techniques, fMRI offers improved spatial and temporal resolution for studying physiologically complex tasks. This study examines differences in brain hemodynamic response between healthy comparison subjects and patients with schizophrenia during performance of a smooth pursuit eye movement task.

Method

Subjects

Fourteen healthy comparison subjects and 14 subjects with DSM-IV schizophrenia participated in this study. Comparison subjects included five men and nine women, with a mean age of 37.6 years (SD=10.6). Subjects with schizophrenia also included five men and nine women, with a mean age of 39.9 years (SD=7.6). No significant difference in age was found between the groups (t=0.51, df=24, p=0.61). Two additional comparison subjects and four additional subjects with schizophrenia were excluded because of excess head motion (>1 mm) during scanning. Two of the 14 subjects with schizophrenia were neuroleptic free. The subjects refrained from smoking for at least 30 minutes before the study. All participants provided written, informed consent according to a procedure approved by the University of Colorado Institutional Review Board.

Task Design

fMR images were obtained while subjects performed a smooth pursuit task (12). The task consisted of visually tracking a small white dot that moved horizontally back and forth over a visual angle of 26° at a constant velocity of 16.7° per second followed by a 700-msec fixation period at the edges. For each of four runs in this blocked design, a 10-second equilibration period was followed by four cycles of 25-second task/25-second rest. During the rest period, the subjects were instructed to look straight ahead at the blank screen.

MR Parameters and Data Analysis

Studies were performed with a 1.5-T Siemens Vision MR system (Siemens AG, Iselin, N.J.) by using a standard quadrature head coil. At the beginning of each study, a high-resolution three-dimensional T1-weighted anatomical scan was acquired (TR=45, TE=20, flip angle=45°, matrix=256×256, field of view=240×240 mm, 1.5-mm thick coronal slices). Functional images were acquired by using gradient-echo echo-planar imaging (TR=2500, TE=50, matrix=64×64, field of view=240×240 mm). Each volume included 20 6-mm thick axial slices angled parallel to the planum sphenoidale. There was a 1-mm gap between slices. Eighty volumes were collected for each of four runs in the study over a 20-minute period.

Quantitative Analyses

Spatial preprocessing, model specification and estimation, and statistical inference were performed with SPM 99 (13). The four runs from each subject were concatenated. Images were motion corrected, normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute template, and subsequently converted to Talairach space (14). A 4-mm full width at half maximum Gaussian smoothing kernel was then applied. After reslicing for motion correction, normalization to stereotactic space, and application of the smoothing kernel, the effective smoothing was approximately 8×8×8 mm3. The model consisted of a simple boxcar function (task versus rest), convolved with a hemodynamic response function.
To account for both between-group and between-subject variation, a random-effects analysis was implemented for the whole brain. Parameter estimates for each individual’s analysis (SPM contrast images) were entered into a second-level analysis consisting of a two-sample t test, contrasting for the conditions of comparison subjects > schizophrenia patients and schizophrenia patients > comparison subjects. The second-level model was thresholded at p<0.05 (t=4.72, df=26), corrected for multiple comparisons by using the false discovery rate method (15).
A supplemental region-of-interest analysis was performed by using the “MarsBar” toolbox in SPM 99 (16). A priori regions consisted of 10-mm-diameter spheres placed in the frontal eye fields, supplementary eye field, parietal eye fields, cingulate gyrus, and occipital region; these areas have previously been shown to be involved in smooth pursuit eye movements in healthy individuals (1719). The mean time course for all voxels in each region was analyzed by using a random-effects analysis as described earlier. Data for each region were thresholded at p<0.05 (t=2.48, df=26), with Bonferroni correction for the number of regions evaluated.

Qualitative Analyses

To assess the variability in activity related to smooth pursuit eye movements in comparison subjects and patients with schizophrenia, a statistical parametric map was created for each subject, as described earlier, and was thresholded at p<0.005, corrected (false discovery rate). Regions previously identified as being involved in smooth pursuit eye movements and regions showing different levels of activation between the groups in this study were evaluated qualitatively. A region was considered to contain “activity” if any above-threshold voxels were present. A chi-square test was performed to determine the significance of between-group differences in activity in those regions.

Head Movement and Voxel-Based Morphometric Analyses

To test for differences in head movement between the groups, realignment parameters from the SPM 99 realignment routine were compared. Movement information included magnitude of movement from the original position in three translational (x-, y-, and z-axes) and three rotational (roll, pitch, and yaw) directions for each of the 320 time points acquired for each subject. No significant between-group differences were found.
A voxel-based morphometric analysis was employed to determine if differences in activation were due to gross differences in anatomy between the groups (20). Group differences were evaluated by using the general linear model in SPM 99 (13), thresholded at p<0.01, and corrected for family-wise error (t=5.91, df=26).

Infrared Oculography

Subjects’ performance on the smooth pursuit eye movement task was assessed outside the magnet with infrared oculography (4) to detect eye movements such as anticipatory saccades and to calculate pursuit gain. Anticipatory saccades are jumps in the direction of target motion, beginning and ending ahead of target location or increasing position error by 100%, followed by a 50-msec interval of eye velocity less than 50% of target velocity. All segments of pursuit were included in the gain calculation so that gain was retained as a global measure of task performance (12). Age-adjusted z scores were computed for each individual (4). The pursuit task was identical to that used in the magnet.

In-Magnet Eye Movement Analysis

To assess compliance with the task while subjects were in the scanner, a power spectrum analysis of the time series data was performed to examine task-correlated fluctuations in voxel intensity in the region of the optic nerve. Time series data were obtained from SPM normalized echo-planar images. Data were averaged from an 8×8×1 voxel region of interest placed over the eye and optic nerve. In order to remove a large zero-order power component and reduce spurious high-frequency power, the mean was subtracted, and a Hamming filter was applied to taper the ends of the data. The spectrum was calculated by using the square of the magnitude of the Fast Fourier Transform. In addition, spectra were normalized by dividing spectrum values by the total power. On the basis of the task paradigm and volume acquisition time, the expected intensity fluctuations caused by eye movement were between 0.16 and 0.2 Hz. Accordingly, the area under the curve of this portion of the spectrum was calculated, and results were compared between fixation and tracking states, as well as between groups (21).

Results

Relative to the comparison subjects, the patients with schizophrenia exhibited greater activity associated with a smooth pursuit eye movement task in multiple brain regions. The patients with schizophrenia demonstrated significantly (p<0.05, corrected) greater activity in the posterior hippocampus bilaterally and in the right fusiform gyrus (Figure 1). A small area of greater activity was also observed in the medial thalamus (Table 1).
No areas of significantly lower activity in the patients with schizophrenia, relative to the comparison subjects, during the pursuit task were observed by using the whole-brain analysis method in this study. Lower activity in the schizophrenia subjects was observed in the frontal eye fields, occipital region, cingulate gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus, but differences in these areas did not reach significance in the stringent random-effects analysis used for the whole-brain analysis.
The region-of-interest analysis examining brain areas known a priori to be involved in smooth pursuit eye movements in healthy individuals revealed lower activity in patients with schizophrenia relative to the comparison subjects in the right frontal eye field, cingulate gyrus, and occipital region (Table 2).
Qualitatively, all comparison subjects and schizophrenia subjects showed pursuit-related activity in the frontal eye fields, parietal eye fields, area V1 and middle temporal visual area/middle superior temporal area, and cerebellum. Most, but not all, subjects demonstrated activation in the supplementary eye field (12 of 14 comparison subjects and 10 of 14 schizophrenia subjects), cingulate gyrus (14 of 14 comparison subjects and 11 of 14 schizophrenia subjects), and thalamus (10 of 14 comparison subjects and 11 of 14 schizophrenia subjects). In contrast, significantly more patients with schizophrenia, relative to the comparison subjects, showed activity in the hippocampus (three of 14 comparison subjects, compared with 11 of 14 schizophrenia subjects, p<0.01, Fisher’s exact test) and fusiform gyrus (four of 14 comparison subjects, compared with 12 of 14 schizophrenia subjects, p<0.01, Fisher’s exact test).
To assess subjects’ eye movements in the scanner, Fourier analyses of echo-planar image time series data from a region of interest covering the eye were performed. This technique proved sensitive to eye movements at the expected frequency of 0.2 Hz (Figure 2). No significant difference between groups in eye movements during the rest (baseline) condition was observed (comparison subjects: mean=289, SD=81.9; schizophrenia subjects: mean=293, SD=66.9) (t=0.14, df=26, p=0.89). A significant group difference was observed during performance of the smooth pursuit task (comparison subjects: mean=896, SD=224; schizophrenia subjects: mean=696, SD=243) (t=2.27, df=26, p<0.03).
The voxel-based morphometric analysis did not reveal any between-group differences in gray matter in regions that showed greater pursuit-related activation in the patients with schizophrenia. Significant (p<0.01, corrected) decreases in the concentration of gray matter in subjects with schizophrenia were observed in the inferior frontal cortex and a small portion of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
Comparison subjects had a mean age-adjusted z score for gain (eye velocity relative to target velocity) of 0.82 (SD=0.44) and a mean age-adjusted z score for percentage of total eye movements due to anticipatory saccades of –0.94 (SD=0.30) (Figure 3). Subjects with schizophrenia had a mean age-adjusted z score for gain of –0.27 (SD=1.24) and a mean age-adjusted z score for percentage of anticipatory saccade of –0.03 (SD=1.02). Eye tracking data were not available for one subject who could not be reached. Both measures of smooth pursuit function were significantly different between the groups (gain: t=3.0, df=15, p<0.009; percentage of anticipatory saccades: t=3.1, df=14, p<0.008).
As an additional exploratory analysis, fMRI data from the schizophrenia patients with the poorest gain (N=6) were compared with data from the patients with the highest gain (N=6) by using a second-level random-effects model as described earlier, thresholded at p<0.005 (t=3.17, df=10). Compared to the group with low gain, the group with high gain showed significantly greater activation in the middle temporal visual area (bilateral) and smaller areas of greater activation in the left insula, left superior frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate, and superior temporal gyrus. Patients with poor gain demonstrated relatively greater activation in the cerebellum. Smaller areas of greater activation in poor trackers were observed in the middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, and posterior cingulate.
A comparison was also made between the comparison subjects and subgroups of patients whose pursuit performance was either similar to or poorer than that of the comparison subjects. Patients whose performance was similar to that of the comparison subjects (N=5) showed robust elevations in posterior hippocampus activity (t=3.36, df=8, p<0.005), relative to the comparison subjects. Smaller elevations in activity were observed in the posterior hippocampi, lateral geniculate nuclei, posterior thalamus, and posterior cerebellum in patients who performed poorly, relative to the comparison subjects (N=7).

Discussion

The major finding of this study is greater pursuit-related activity in the posterior hippocampus and right fusiform gyrus in patients with schizophrenia, relative to healthy comparison subjects. In addition, a region-of-interest analysis of areas known to be involved in smooth pursuit revealed differences in frontal areas, including lower activation in the frontal eye fields, cingulate gyrus, and occipital region.
The involvement of the hippocampus and fusiform gyrus in eye movement tasks has been demonstrated with single-unit recordings in primates (22, 23) and with PET studies in humans (24, 25). Differential activation of these regions during a smooth pursuit task in schizophrenia is consistent with neuropathological and neurophysiological evidence for inhibitory dysfunction of these regions, particularly the hippocampus, in the disease (26, 27). The finding of greater hippocampal activation in patients with schizophrenia during a smooth pursuit eye movement task in this study suggests that abnormalities in their eye movements may also involve failure of inhibition in the hippocampus. Greater hippocampal activation in schizophrenia patients whose pursuit performance was either poorer than or similar to that of the comparison subjects suggests that hyperactivation of this region may not be associated with pursuit quality but rather with a diagnosis of schizophrenia.
Our findings also suggest involvement of other brain regions in smooth pursuit dysfunction in schizophrenia. Region-of-interest analysis of smooth pursuit-related areas revealed differences in frontal function in the schizophrenia patients. Specifically, the lower activation of frontal eye field observed in this study is consistent with previous PET studies demonstrating lower pursuit-associated activity in the frontal eye fields in relatives of patients with schizophrenia (10) and lower resting frontal eye field glucose metabolism in schizophrenia (10, 11, 28). Frontal eye field dysfunction may be particularly important in the low gain component of pursuit dysfunction in schizophrenia (8). Frontal regions such as the frontal eye fields and cingulate gyrus may also play a role in the saccadic intrusion component of smooth pursuit abnormalities (29). In a recent study by Rosano and colleagues (30), high-resolution imaging at 3 T was used to distinguish subregions of the frontal eye fields that were related to smooth pursuit from subregions related to saccades. While the resolution used in the present study limits our discussion to the larger functional area, the interpretation of lower activity in this region contributing to greater saccadic intrusion is consistent with a recent human neurosurgical mapping study by Milea and colleagues (31), in which the investigators stimulated the frontal eye fields with electrodes. Frontal eye field stimulation elicited spontaneous contraversive smooth pursuit and interfered with saccadic eye movements. It seems reasonable to speculate, therefore, that a gross deficit in activity in this region may lead to lower quality of smooth pursuit caused by a greater number of intrusive saccades.
In addition to the involvement of frontal areas in pursuit dysfunction, this study demonstrated the involvement of posterior visual processing areas such as the fusiform gyrus. This region is known to be involved in higher visual processing, particularly for faces and other shape recognition (32). While the fusiform has received a great deal of attention for its role in the processing of faces, and it can be functionally divided into anterior and posterior regions based on this function, less is known about its role in processing other visual information. The region shown to be differentially activated in this study has been shown to be active in healthy comparison subjects during passive viewing of a field of many horizontally moving stimuli (square blocks, very similar to the smooth pursuit stimulus used in the present study), compared to fixation (33), suggesting that this subregion of the fusiform gyrus may play a role in processing motion information. Malaspina and colleagues (34) demonstrated increased cerebral blood flow in the right fusiform during visual fixation in schizophrenia.
Primary sensory and motor areas may also be involved in pursuit dysfunction in schizophrenia. In the within-group analysis comparing schizophrenia patients with high versus low gain, the middle temporal visual area, which is known to be involved in motion processing, was significantly more active in the good trackers. This finding may be consistent with the hypothesis of impaired velocity discrimination in schizophrenia (9). In addition, the posterior cerebellum was more active in the poor trackers, relative to both the good trackers with schizophrenia and to the comparison subjects. This region plays a role in coordinating saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movement signals from the frontal cortex, and it has been suggested that this region is involved in pursuit dysfunction in schizophrenia (35).
One limitation of this study is that we are not able to exclude the possibility that observed differences may be related to differences in regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) between the groups (3638). It is also difficult to exclude neuroleptic effects. It has been shown, however, that typical neuroleptics do not alter smooth pursuit performance (39) or rCBF in patients with schizophrenia (38). Smooth pursuit-associated brain activation patterns in the two neuroleptic-free patients in this study were qualitatively similar to those observed in the medicated patients.
Finally, it is possible that differences in brain activation may have been due to differential performance in the baseline rest condition, in which all subjects were instructed to “look straight ahead” at the black screen. This explanation seems unlikely, however, as the Fourier analysis of eye movement detected in the echo-planar images revealed no significant difference in baseline performance. In addition, to test for the possibility of group differences in brain activation during rest, we explicitly contrasted for both higher and lower levels of activity between groups during rest and found no differences (data not shown).
In conclusion, this study has identified temporal and frontal lobe differences in brain activity associated with a smooth pursuit eye movement task in schizophrenia. The largest observed difference was a relatively higher level of activity in the bilateral posterior hippocampus in the subjects with schizophrenia. This observation is consistent with the hypothesis that inhibitory interneurons in the hippocampus are dysfunctional in schizophrenia. Differences in the fusiform gyrus and frontal regions were also observed. The relatively hyperactive temporal and hypoactive frontal regions observed in this study are consistent with the hypothesis of abnormal functional connectivity between these two brain areas in schizophrenia (40).
TABLE 1
TABLE 2
Figure 1. fMRI Images Showing Brain Regions in Which Subjects With Schizophrenia (N=14) Had Greater Activation During Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements Than Healthy Comparison Subjects (N=14)a
aGreater activation was found in the bilateral posterior hippocampi and right fusiform gyrus in the subjects with schizophrenia, relative to the comparison subjects. Statistical maps thresholded at p<0.05, corrected.
Figure 2. Fourier Analysis of Echo-Planar fMRI Time Series Data From a Voxel in the Region of the Optic Nerve During Smooth Pursuit Eye Movements in Patients With Schizophrenia (N=14) and Healthy Comparison Subjects (N=14)a
aThe echo-planar image shows the location of the voxel in the region of the optic nerve. The Fourier analysis showed sensitivity to eye movements at the expected frequency (0.16–0.21 Hz) in both groups.
Figure 3. Scores for Gain and Anticipatory Saccades During Smooth Pursuit Eye Movement in Healthy Comparison Subjects (N=14) and Patients With Schizophrenia (N=13)a
aThe bars represent mean values. Subjects with schizophrenia had significantly lower gain (t=3.0, df=15, p<0.009) and significantly greater anticipatory saccade frequency (t=3.1, df=14, p<0.08).

Footnote

Received March 11, 2003; revision received July 15, 2003; accepted July 21, 2003. From the Departments of Pharmacology, Radiology, and Psychiatry, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Denver. Address reprint requests to Dr. Tregellas, University of Colorado Health Sciences Center, Campus Box C268-71, 4200 East 9th Ave., Denver, CO 80262; [email protected] (e-mail). Supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs Research Service and NIMH grants MH-38321 and MH-66533-01.

References

1.
Levy DL, Holzman PS, Matthysse S, Mendell NR: Eye tracking dysfunction and schizophrenia: a critical perspective. Schizophr Bull 1993; 19:461–536
2.
Hommer DW, Clem T, Litman R, Pickar D: Maladaptive anticipatory saccades in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1991; 30:779–794
3.
Rosenberg DR, Sweeney JA, Squires-Wheeler E, Keshavan MS, Cornblatt BA, Erlenmeyer-Kimling L: Eye-tracking dysfunction in offspring from the New York High-Risk Project: diagnostic specificity and the role of attention. Psychiatry Res 1997; 66:121–130
4.
Ross RG, Olincy A, Mikulich SK, Radant AD, Harris JG, Waldo M, Compagnon N, Heinlein S, Leonard S, Zerbe GO, Adler L, Freedman R: Admixture analysis of smooth pursuit eye movements in probands with schizophrenia and their relatives suggests gain and leading saccades are potential endophenotypes. Psychophysiology 2002; 39:809–819
5.
Ross RG, Olincy A, Radant A: Amplitude criteria and anticipatory saccades during smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia. Psychophysiology 1999; 36:464–468
6.
Clementz BA, Reid SA, McDowell JE, Cadenhead KS: Abnormality of smooth pursuit eye movement initiation: specificity to the schizophrenia spectrum? Psychophysiology 1995; 32:130–134
7.
Sweeney JA, Clementz BA, Haas GL, Escobar MD, Drake K, Frances AJ: Eye tracking dysfunction in schizophrenia: characterization of component eye movement abnormalities, diagnostic specificity, and the role of attention. J Abnorm Psychol 1994; 103:222–230
8.
Sweeney JA, Luna B, Srinivasagam NM, Keshavan MS, Schooler NR, Haas GL, Carl JR: Eye tracking abnormalities in schizophrenia: evidence for dysfunction in the frontal eye fields. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 44:698–708
9.
Chen Y, Nakayama K, Levy DL, Matthysse S, Holzman PS: Psychophysical isolation of a motion-processing deficit in schizophrenics and their relatives and its association with impaired smooth pursuit. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1999; 96:4724–4729
10.
O’Driscoll GA, Benkelfat C, Florencio PS, Wolff AL, Joober R, Lal S, Evans AC: Neural correlates of eye tracking deficits in first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients: a positron emission tomography study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999; 56:1127–1134
11.
Ross DE, Thaker GK, Holcomb HH, Cascella NG, Medoff DR, Tamminga CA: Abnormal smooth pursuit eye movements in schizophrenic patients are associated with cerebral glucose metabolism in oculomotor regions. Psychiatry Res 1995; 58:53–67
12.
Radant AD, Hommer DW: A quantitative analysis of saccades and smooth pursuit during visual pursuit tracking: a comparison of schizophrenics with normals and substance abusing controls. Schizophr Res 1992; 6:225–235
13.
Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Worsley KJ, Poline JB, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS: Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: a general linear approach. Hum Brain Mapp 1995; 2:189–210
14.
Talairach J, Tournoux P: Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: Three-Dimensional Proportional System. New York, Thieme Medical, 1988
15.
Genovese CR, Lazar NA, Nichols T: Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate. Neuroimage 2002; 15:870–878
16.
Brett M, Anton J, Valabregue R, Poline J: Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox (Proceedings of the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain). Neuroimage 2002; 16(2), abstract 497
17.
Berman RA, Colby CL, Genovese CR, Voyvodic JT, Luna B, Thulborn KR, Sweeney JA: Cortical networks subserving pursuit and saccadic eye movements in humans: an fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 1999; 8:209–225
18.
Petit L, Clark VP, Ingeholm J, Haxby JV: Dissociation of saccade-related and pursuit-related activation in human frontal eye fields as revealed by fMRI. J Neurophysiol 1997; 77:3386–3390
19.
Tanabe J, Tregellas J, Miller D, Ross RG, Freedman R: Brain activation during smooth-pursuit eye movements. Neuroimage 2002; 17:1315–1324
20.
Ashburner J, Friston KJ: Voxel-based morphometry—the methods. Neuroimage 2000; 11:805–821
21.
Tregellas JR, Tanabe JL, Miller DE, Freedman R: Monitoring eye movements during fMRI tasks with echo planar images. Hum Brain Mapp 2002; 17:237–243
22.
Ringo JL, Sobotka S, Diltz MD, Bunce CM: Eye movements modulate activity in hippocampal, parahippocampal, and inferotemporal neurons. J Neurophysiol 1994; 71:1285–1288
23.
Sobotka S, Nowicka A, Ringo JL: Activity linked to externally cued saccades in single units recorded from hippocampal, parahippocampal, and inferotemporal areas of macaques. J Neurophysiol 1997; 78:2156–2163
24.
Hasebe H, Oyamada H, Kinomura S, Kawashima R, Ouchi Y, Nobezawa S, Tsukada H, Yoshikawa E, Ukai K, Takada R, Takagi M, Abe H, Fukuda H, Bando T: Human cortical areas activated in relation to vergence eye movements—a PET study. Neuroimage 1999; 10:200–208
25.
Petit L, Orssaud C, Tzourio N, Salamon G, Mazoyer B, Berthoz A: PET study of voluntary saccadic eye movements in humans: basal ganglia-thalamocortical system and cingulate cortex involvement. J Neurophysiol 1993; 69:1009–1017
26.
Benes FM, Kwok EW, Vincent SL, Todtenkopf MS: A reduction of nonpyramidal cells in sector CA2 of schizophrenics and manic depressives. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 44:88–97
27.
Freedman R, Adler LE, Bickford P, Byerley W, Coon H, Cullum CM, Griffith JM, Harris JG, Leonard S, Miller C: Schizophrenia and nicotinic receptors. Harv Rev Psychiatry 1994; 2:179–192
28.
Blackwood DH, Ebmeier KP, Muir WJ, Sharp CW, Glabus M, Walker M, Souza V, Dunan JR, Goodwin GM: Correlation of regional cerebral blood flow equivalents measured by single photon emission computerized tomography with P300 latency and eye movement abnormality in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1994; 90:157–166
29.
Pierrot-Deseilligny C: Saccade and smooth-pursuit impairment after cerebral hemispheric lesions. Eur Neurol 1994; 34:121–134
30.
Rosano C, Krisky CM, Welling JS, Eddy WF, Luna B, Thulborn KR, Sweeney JA: Pursuit and saccadic eye movement subregions in human frontal eye field: a high-resolution fMRI investigation. Cereb Cortex 2002; 12:107–115
31.
Milea D, Lobel E, Lehericy S, Duffau H, Rivaud-Pechoux S, Berthoz A, Pierrot-Deseilligny C: Intraoperative frontal eye field stimulation elicits ocular deviation and saccade suppression. Neuroreport 2002; 13:1359–1364
32.
Kanwisher N: Domain specificity in face perception. Nat Neurosci 2000; 3:759–763
33.
Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Dobmeyer S, Shulman GL, Petersen SE: Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography. J Neurosci 1991; 11:2383–2402
34.
Malaspina D, Storer S, Furman V, Esser P, Printz D, Berman A, Lignelli A, Gorman J, Van Heertum R: SPECT study of visual fixation in schizophrenia and comparison subjects. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 46:89–93
35.
Ross RG, Olincy A, Harris JG, Radant A, Adler LE, Freedman R: Anticipatory saccades during smooth pursuit eye movements and familial transmission of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 1998; 44:690–697
36.
Franck N, O’Leary DS, Flaum M, Hichwa RD, Andreasen NC: Cerebral blood flow changes associated with Schneiderian first-rank symptoms in schizophrenia. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002; 14:277–282
37.
Schultz SK, O’Leary DS, Boles Ponto LL, Arndt S, Magnotta V, Watkins GL, Hichwa RD, Andreasen NC: Age and regional cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia: age effects in anterior cingulate, frontal, and parietal cortex. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2002; 14:19–24
38.
Yildiz A, Eryilmaz M, Gungor F, Erkilic M, Karayalcin B: Regional cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia before and after neuroleptic medication. Nucl Med Commun 2000; 21:1113–1118
39.
Litman RE, Hommer DW, Radant A, Clem T, Pickar D: Quantitative effects of typical and atypical neuroleptics on smooth pursuit eye tracking in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1994; 12:107–120
40.
Fletcher P, McKenna PJ, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Dolan RJ: Abnormal cingulate modulation of fronto-temporal connectivity in schizophrenia. Neuroimage 1999; 9:337–342

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 315 - 321
PubMed: 14754781

History

Published online: 1 February 2004
Published in print: February 2004

Authors

Details

Jason R. Tregellas, Ph.D.
David E. Miller, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share