Skip to main content
Full access
Editorial
Published Online: 1 February 2004

Choose to Study Choice in Addiction

Publication: American Journal of Psychiatry
Awareness that addiction is a disease of the brain, and that addiction can in part be defined as having little control over craving and relapse, is an accomplishment arising from decades of clinical and preclinical research. However, with the emergence of a new political environment over the last few years, the question of addiction as a personal choice is again being asked by national and local policy makers. This has rekindled interest by biomedical researchers and clinicians to define the point at which the ability to make socially correct decisions is compromised by the disease of addiction. The most simple approach is to say that initial experimentation is a choice, but the ability to choose is thereafter compromised. While this view is in need of refinement, since the majority of Americans who have experimented with legal and illicit drugs of abuse made a choice to quit or limit drug intake, it is clear that factors associated with addiction limit the addict’s ability to make adaptive decisions about drug use. These factors include genetic vulnerability, pharmacologically induced neuroadaptations produced by repeated drug use, and socioeconomic variables. Researchers are actively quantifying each of these factors, and knowledge along these lines continues to lead to improvements in therapeutic options for addicts. However, this critical research effort does not directly address the neurobiological mechanisms underlying the capacity of a person to make a choice about drug use, nor does it address questions such as when during a developing addiction disorder does decision-making ability become compromised, or when with abstinence might the ability to make socially adaptive decisions partly recover.
Two articles in this issue of the Journal indirectly provide clues as to how to begin to evaluate the neurobiology of the compromised executive functions responsible for making poor choices about addictive substances. Both studies involve functional neuroimaging of addicts. The study by Wang et al. examines glucose metabolism with PET in methamphetamine addicts following short (<6 months) and protracted (12-17 months) intervals of abstinence and asks the question whether there is recovery of the deficits observed in glucose metabolism after short methamphetamine withdrawal. The authors make the interesting observation that differential recovery occurs, with the reductions in the striatum being invariant between withdrawal periods, but the reductions in the thalamus showing recovery. The increased glucose metabolism in the thalamus was associated with improved performance in motor and memory tasks. The authors speculate that the lack of improvement in striatal metabolism reflects a relatively permanent loss of dopamine innervation (1), while improvement in the thalamus reflects enduring compensatory adaptations in striatal-thalamic circuitry. These data demonstrate significant changes in functional brain circuitry in order to improve behavioral responding in methamphetamine addicts. Whether these adaptations reflect an improvement in decision making was not discerned. In fact, it could be argued that compensatory adaptations in striatal-thalamic circuitry might lead to reliance by an addict more on habit than executive functioning, which is thought to involve cortical areas such as the cognitive subdivision of the anterior cingulate (24). Nonetheless, this study points toward a need to examine changes in circuitry downstream from the pharmacological site of drug action for adaptations that compromise decision-making ability in addicts or may contribute to restoration of decision making following extended abstinence.
The second article in the current issue by Kilts et al. provides data more directly applicable to the issue of choice in addiction. Using PET imaging, this group asked the question of whether there is a gender difference in regional cerebral blood flow in cocaine addicts and postulated that women would exhibit less limbic activation in agreement with less cue-induced craving (5). The authors made the striking observation that in contrast with the long-held view that increased blood flow to the amygdala is coupled with the induction of cue-related cocaine craving (4), women show substantially less increase than men in blood flow to the amygdala. Because of a near significant negative correlation in blood flow between the amygdala and dorsal anterior cingulate, Kilts et al. suggest that this may reflect a greater degree of executive control over cue valuation by the amygdala. Thus, this study may contain the rudiments of a neuroimaging assay for the ability to self-regulate craving and presumably relapse to drug taking.
While neither of the studies directly assessed the problem of choice in addiction, both demonstrate that the technical ability may exist to quantify the ability of addicts to regulate neuronal and thereby behavioral responses to relapse-provoking cues. Studies designed specifically along these lines could provide the potential to evaluate decision-making abilities with regard to drug associations. This information can then be used to guide therapeutic options for treatment depending on how biologically compromised the addict’s decision-making ability has become as a result of genetic disposition, drug-induced neuroadaptations, and socioeconomic variables.

Footnote

Address reprint requests to Dr. Kalivas, Department of Physiology and Neuroscience, Medical University of South Carolina, 173 Ashley Ave., BSB 403, Charleston, SC 29425.

References

1.
Wilson JM, Kalasinsky KS, Levey AI, Bergeron C, Reiber G, Anthony RM, Schmunk GA, Shannak K, Haycock JW, Kish SJ: Striatal dopamine nerve terminal markers in human, chronic methamphetamine users. Nat Med 1996; 2:699–703
2.
Bush G, Vogt BA, Holmes J, Dale AM, Greve D, Jenike MA, Rosen BR: Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: a role in reward-based decision making. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002; 99:523–528
3.
Beauregard M, Levesque J, Bourouin P: Neuronal correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. J Neurosci 2001; 21:RC165
4.
Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND: Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159:1642–1652
5.
Robbins SJ, Ehrman RN, Childress AR, O’Brien CP: Comparing levels of cocaine cue reactivity in male and female outpatients. Drug Alcohol Depend 1999; 53:223–230

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 193 - 194
PubMed: 14754764

History

Published online: 1 February 2004
Published in print: February 2004

Authors

Details

Peter W. Kalivas, Ph.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share