Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective:

Exposure to antenatal maternal anxiety and complex genetic variations may shape fetal brain development. In particular, the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene, located on chromosome 22q11.2, regulates catecholamine signaling in the prefrontal cortex and is implicated in anxiety, pain, and stress responsivity. This study examined whether individual single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) of the COMT gene and their haplotypes moderate the association between antenatal maternal anxiety and in utero cortical development.

Method:

A total of 146 neonates were genotyped and underwent MRI shortly after birth. Neonatal cortical morphology was characterized using cortical thickness. Antenatal maternal anxiety was assessed using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory at week 26 of pregnancy.

Results:

Individual COMT SNPs (val158met, rs737865, and rs165599) modulated the association between antenatal maternal anxiety and the prefrontal and parietal cortical thickness in neonates. Based on haplotype trend regression analysis, findings also showed that among rs737865-val158met-rs165599 haplotypes, the A-val-G (AGG) haplotype probabilities modulated positive associations of antenatal maternal anxiety with cortical thickness in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the right superior parietal cortex and precuneus. In contrast, the G-met-A (GAA) haplotype probabilities modulated negative associations of antenatal maternal anxiety with cortical thickness in bilateral precentral gyrus and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.

Conclusions:

These results suggest that the association between maternal anxiety and in utero neurodevelopment is modified through complex genetic variation in COMT. Such genetic moderation may explain, in part, the variation in phenotypic outcomes in offspring associated with maternal emotional well-being.
Anxiety has a strong familial component (1). Children of affected parents show an increased risk for emotional problems compared with the normal population (2). While the precise mechanism through which vulnerability is transmitted remains unclear, there is evidence that antenatal maternal emotional well-being associates with alterations in the uterine environment (3) and in fetal physiology (4). Antenatal maternal anxiety and the accompanying alterations in the uterine environment also predict an increased risk for childhood behavioral and emotional problems (3), decreased gray matter density of prefrontal and sensory cortices in childhood (5, 6), and alterations in performance on prefrontal dependent tasks (5). Importantly, the associations of antenatal maternal anxiety with childhood emotional function (7) and early brain growth, particularly in structures that are implicated in the regulation of emotional states (8), are independent of postnatal maternal anxiety. These findings appear to reflect a prenatal, transgenerational transmission of individual differences in vulnerability for emotional problems.
Vulnerability for anxiety disorders in children of affected parents may also be moderated through genetic variations (9). The catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) gene, located on chromosome 22q11.2, is well expressed across the brain before birth. The COMT gene starts its expression in the prefrontal cortex at infancy and peaks during early adulthood (10, 11). It regulates catecholamine signaling, particularly dopamine signaling, in the prefrontal cortex and is associated with the development of the prefrontal anatomy and function (12). It has been suggested that COMT variants are molecular genetic markers associated with anxiety, pain, and stress responsivity (13). A functional single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) of the COMT gene (rs4680) leads to an amino acid change from valine (val) to methionine (met) at position 158 (val158met) (14). The val allele is suggested as a predominant factor that determines higher COMT activity in the prefrontal cortex, which presumably leads to lower synaptic dopamine levels through enhanced degradation (15). The COMT genotype is associated with morphological changes of prefrontal, temporal, and superior parietal regions in neonates (16) and adolescents (17). The COMT val polymorphism associates with altered processing of emotional stimuli in brain regions such as the prefrontal cortex and amygdala, which are implicated in anxiety disorders (18). Thus, the COMT val polymorphism is linked to an increased risk for early-onset major depression (19), panic disorders (20), and anxiety in adolescents and adults (9, 21). Recently, Sheikh et al. (22) found that even during early childhood, children homozygous for the val allele show higher levels of internalizing symptoms compared with children with at least one copy of the met allele. Interestingly, some studies based on Asian populations did not replicate the association of the COMT val allele with anxiety-related disorders (23). Moreover, Baumann et al. (9) found that individuals homozygous for the COMT met allele with adverse childhood experiences show higher anxiety sensitivity, which is also supported by findings from the meta-analysis conducted by Domschke et al. (24). One reason for this discrepancy could be the result of differences in populations.
In addition to val158met, sequence variants lying in other loci within the COMT gene affect gene function, ultimately resulting in altered enzyme activity and adding complexity to the functional and clinical implications of COMT variations. Variation of the COMT val158met (rs4680) polymorphism with the upstream SNP in intron 1 (rs737865), and the downstream SNP in the 3′ untranslated region (rs165599), has been implicated in anxiety (2527). This three-SNP haplotype (rs737865-val158met-rs165599) differentially alters the expression of the val158met alleles in human brain tissue (14). In addition, variations of this haplotype show a statistically stronger impact on prefrontal functions than the three individual SNPs, suggesting the importance of complex interactions of functional variations in COMT in the human brain in anxiety (28). A COMT haplotype comprised of val158met and rs737865 associates with high neuroticism and low extraversion, which are characteristics of anxiety-prone individuals (26).
A prospective, longitudinal birth mother-offspring cohort study, GUSTO (Growing Up in Singapore Toward Health Outcomes), provided a unique opportunity to examine whether COMT genotype moderates the association between antenatal maternal anxiety and in utero brain development. In this study, neonatal brain morphology was characterized by cortical thickness measured from the T2-weighted MRI of the brain acquired shortly after birth. We predicted that individual SNPs of the COMT gene (rs737865, val158met, and rs165599) would modulate the associations between antenatal maternal anxiety and cortical thickness in neonates, particularly in the prefrontal cortex. Furthermore, we predicted that haplotype analysis would be superior in predicting such effects compared with individual SNPs. We first estimated the rs737865-val158met-rs165599 haplotype for each participant through an expectation-maximization algorithm (29). We then explored haplotype trend regression analysis on the neuroimaging data, where estimated haplotype probabilities were used to increase statistical power and generate findings unbiased to samples with uniquely determined haplotypes. The present study provides, to our knowledge, the first direct analysis of the genetic and environmental interactions associated with neonatal brain morphology and evidence that COMT may represent a molecular genetic marker moderating differential sensitivity to antenatal maternal anxiety.

Method

Participants

One-hundred and eighty-nine infants of mothers who participated in the prospective GUSTO birth cohort study were recruited for neuroimaging. The GUSTO cohort consisted of pregnant Asian women attending the first-trimester antenatal ultrasound scan clinic at the National University Hospital and the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital in Singapore. Birth outcome and pregnancy measures were obtained from hospital records. The parents were Singapore citizens or permanent residents of Chinese, Malay, or Indian ethnic background. Socioeconomic status (household income) was extracted from survey questionnaires during pregnancy. This study only includes neonates with gestational age ≥37 weeks, birth weight >2,500 g, and a 5-minute Apgar score ≥9. The GUSTO cohort study was approved by the National Healthcare Group Domain Specific Review Board and the SingHealth Centralized Institutional Review Board. Written consent was obtained from the mothers.

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory Scale

The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y-2) was administered at 26 weeks of pregnancy to quantify the maternal prenatal level of anxiety. The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory is a commonly used self-report tool for assessing anxiety and consists of two subscales (state and trait anxiety), each containing 20 items on a 4-point rating scale. State anxiety reflects a “transitory emotional state or condition of the human organism that is characterized by subjective, consciously perceived feelings of tension and apprehension and heightened autonomic nervous system activity.” State anxiety may fluctuate over time and can vary in intensity. In contrast, trait anxiety denotes “relatively stable individual differences in anxiety proneness” and refers to a general tendency to respond with anxiety to perceived threats in the environment.” Items that assess the absence of anxiety are reverse scored. The state and trait anxiety scores can be directly computed as the sum of scores of each item and range from 20 to 80. They have a direct interpretation: high scores on their respective scales mean more trait or state anxiety, and low scores mean less. In regard to the perinatal population, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory has been shown to have construct validity (30). The reliability of this screening scale was 0.91 for both the state and trait subscales assessed using Cronbach’s analysis for our cohort. Several studies have suggested that maternal anxiety trait tends to remain relatively stable over the course of pregnancy (31, 32). Hence, only the trait measure at 26 weeks of pregnancy was used in this study to examine the prenatal maternal anxiety.

MRI Acquisition and Analysis

One-hundred and eighty-nine neonates underwent axial fast spin-echo T2-weighted MRI (repetition time=3,500 ms; echo time=110 ms; field of view=256 mm×256 mm; matrix size=256×256; 50 axial slices with 2.0-mm thickness) at 5 to 17 days of life using a 1.5-Tesla GE scanner (General Electric Co., Fairfield, Conn.) at the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging of the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital. We obtained two acquisitions of the axial T2-weighted MRI while participants were sleeping in the scanner. No sedation was used, and precautions were taken to reduce exposure to the MRI scanner noise. A neonatologist was present during each scan. A pulse oximeter was used to monitor heart rate and oxygen saturation throughout the entire scan. All brain scans were reviewed by a neuroradiologist (M.V.F). To determine image motion, all axial slices of the T2-weighted MRI data were visually inspected to ensure no cross-slice motion and checkerboard patterns.
Within individual participants, when possible, two T2-weighted MRI acquisitions were first rigidly aligned and averaged to increase signal-to-noise ratio. In cases in which only one scan was acquired, data from one scan were used in lieu of the average axial image. The skull of the averaged axial image was removed using the Brain Extraction Tool method (33). A Markov random field model was used to automatically delineate gray matter, white matter, and CSF from the neonatal T2-weighted MRI data. The mathematical model of Markov random field has been described in detail by Fischl et al. (34). The Markov random field model has been considered as one of robust automatic brain segmentation approaches because it incorporates the anatomical prior information obtained from a manually labeled training set. In our study, our training set consisted of 20 T2-weighted MRI data sets randomly selected from our sample. We employed the leave-one-out validation approach to evaluate the Markov random field segmentation accuracy for gray and white matter. The 19 images with the manual label were used as training sets in Markov random field, and one image with the manual label was used as a testing set. The accuracies of the Markov random field segmentation for gray and white matter were 0.793 and 0.862, respectively. An example of the infant brain image and its segmentation is presented in Figure 1.
FIGURE 1. Illustration of Infant T2-Weighted Image and Segmentation
Based on the above tissue segmentation, a cortical surface was constructed at the boundary between gray and white matter using a graph-based topology correction algorithm (35). The cortical thickness was measured as the distance between the cortical surface and gray matter voxels at the boundary between gray matter and CSF. The cortical thickness was smoothed using the Laplace-Beltrami basis functions on the cortical surface. For group comparison of the cortical thickness, we employed a large deformation diffeomorphic metric mapping algorithm (36) to align individual cortical surfaces to the atlas that was generated based on the cortical anatomy of the same 20 participants (37, 38) and transferred the thickness of each individual participant to the atlas.

SNP Genotyping

Genomic DNA was extracted from frozen umbilical cord specimens for infants and from blood for mothers according to standard procedures. Then, the samples were genotyped on Illumina Omni express arrays (Illumina, San Diego), which were recently shown to perform well and have better coverage compared with competitors in Asian populations (39), and on Illumina Exome arrays following the manufacturer’s instructions by Expression Analysis, Inc. (Durham, N.C.). Data were processed in the GenomeStudio Genotyping module (Illumina, San Diego). Genotyping calls were made using the GenCall software (Illumina, San Diego), which incorporates a clustering algorithm (GenTrain) and a calling algorithm (Bayesian model). The GenCall score of each SNP probe and call rate of each sample were generated. The GenCall score is primarily designed as a means by which to rank and filter out failed genotypes (40). Scores below 0.15 generally indicate failed genotypes, and hence the genotypes with a GenCall score <0.15 are not assigned genotypes (40). COMT SNPs rs4680, rs737865, and rs165599 were then extracted and used in this study. Their minor allele frequencies were >0.05. The three SNPs were also not deviated from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (p<0.001).

Linkage Disequilibrium and Haplotype Estimation

To measure linkage disequilibrium between SNPs, we estimated the haplotype frequencies through use of the expectation-maximization algorithm on individual genotypes and calculated Lewontin’s D′ and Pearson’s correlation using Haploview software, version 4.2 (41). We then constructed likely three-SNP haplotypes in our samples on the basis of the three polymorphisms using PHASE software, version 2.1.1 (29). Only haplotypes with an estimated frequency >3% were used in the analysis with imaging.

Statistical Analysis

The thickness data were smoothed on the cortical surface using a 30-mm full-width-at-half-maximum Gaussian filter (42). Linear regressions were first used to examine interactive effects of individual SNPs with antenatal anxiety on cortical thickness. The antenatal maternal State-Trait Anxiety Inventory score and the genotype of individual SNPs were included as main factors, which were entered into the first block of linear regressions, along with sex, birth weight adjusted by gestational age, and postconceptual age on the day of MRI. Monthly household income and ethnicity were separately entered into the first block to avoid multicollinearity. The interaction of the two main factors was then entered into the second block of the regression. Statistical results at each surface vertex were thresholded at the level of significance (p<0.001) and then corrected for multiple comparisons at the cluster level of significance (p<0.05). The significance of each cluster was determined using random field theory as described by Chung et al. (43). Additional analysis was performed while controlling for the maternal genotype. Post hoc analyses were further used to examine associations of antenatal maternal anxiety and cortical thickness in each genotype group, where cortical thickness was computed as mean thickness in the brain region with the significant interactive effects.
Haplotype trend regressions were then used to test for interactive effects of haplotypes and antenatal anxiety on cortical thickness (44). The haplotypes were represented by their estimated frequency of individual participants using PHASE software (29). The other factors in the regression were the same as mentioned above. Statistical results were corrected for multiple comparisons at the cluster level of significance (p<0.05).
Birth weight was adjusted based on gestational age, which was determined using linear regression, with the mean-centered gestational age as a main factor in the GUSTO cohort. The residual was defined as adjusted birth weight that is statistically independent of gestational age.

Results

Demographic Characteristics

Among 189 neonates who underwent MRI scans, our study excluded neonates with gestational age <37 weeks (N=13), with birth weight <2,500 g (N=11), with a 5-minute Apgar score <9 (N=2), with large motion on the T2-weighted MRI (N=5), with no maternal anxiety measure (N=4), and with no genetic data (N=8). Thus, the total sample examined in this study was 146.
Demographic characteristics of mothers and infants are presented in Table 1. Antenatal maternal anxiety did not vary as a function of infant sex (t=–0.39, df=144, p=0.70) or ethnicity (F=1.88, df=2, 143, p=0.16). Importantly, antenatal maternal anxiety also did not differ as a function of infant genotype: rs737685 (F=1.87, df=2, 143, p=0.16), val158met (F=0.66, df=2, 143, p=0.52), rs165599 (F=0.27, df=2, 143, p=0.85), showing that maternal anxiety was independent of infant COMT genotypes. However, antenatal maternal anxiety was negatively correlated with monthly household income (Pearson's r=–0.37, df=146, p<0.001). Monthly household income was highly correlated with ethnicity (Pearson’s r=–0.23, df=146, p=0.01). To avoid multicollinearity in regression analysis, monthly household income and ethnicity were separately entered into regression analysis as covariates.
TABLE 1. Demographic Characteristics of Participants (N=146)
CharacteristicMeanSDN%
Gestational age (weeks)38.81.0  
Birth weight (gram)3,167.7352  
Maternal anxiety score37.69.2  
Male sex  7551.4
Ethnicity    
 Chinese  6544.5
 Malay  5739.0
 Indian  2416.4
Monthly household income (U.S. dollars)    
 ≤$768  64.1
 $769–$1,538  2013.7
 $1,538–S3,076  5537.7
 $3,077–$4,615  3221.9
 ≥$4,616  2215.1
 Unreported  117.5

COMT and Haplotypes

All alleles were in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium with minor allele frequency >0.05 in our sample. Distributions of the genotypes for the three SNPs among mothers and infants are presented in Table 2. Among Chinese infants, there were 46 with AA, 15 with AG, and four with GG of rs737865; there were five met homozygotes, 26 with met-val, and 34 val homozygotes of val158met; there were 11 with AA, 34 with AG, and 19 with GG of rs165599 (data were missing for one infant). Among Malay infants, there were 33 with AA, 23 with AG, and one with GG of rs737865; there were four met homozygotes, 22 with met-val, and 31 val homozygotes of val158met; there were 15 with AA, 23 with AG, and 19 with GG of rs165599 (data were missing for one infant). Among Indian infants, there were 12 with AA and 12 with AG of rs737865; there were five met homozygotes, 12 with met-val, and seven val homozygotes of val158met; there were seven with AA, 11 with AG, and six with GG of rs165599. Although meta-analysis suggested differences in the COMT genotype distribution among different ethnic groups (24), our study did not reveal ethnic group differences in the distributions of rs737865 (χ2=8.93, df=4, p=0.06), val158met2=6.76, df=4, p=0.15), and rs165599 (χ2=4.34, df=4, p=0.63).
TABLE 2. Genotype and Estimated Haplotype Frequencies Among Mothers and Neonates (N=146)
GenotypeGenotype (%)Estimated Haplotype Frequency
Mean (%)SD (%)
Mothers   
rs737865 (A, G)   
 AA61  
 AG37  
 GG2  
rs4680 (val158met; A-met, G-val)   
 AA8  
 AG36  
 GG56  
rs165599 (A, G)   
 AA27  
 AG40  
 GG33  
Infants   
rs737865 (A, G)   
 AA62  
 AG34  
 GG4  
rs4680 (val158met; A-met, G-val)   
 AA10  
 AG41  
 GG49  
rs165599 (A, G)   
 AA23  
 AG47  
 GG30  
Haplotype   
AGG 421.18
AGA 121.19
AAG 40.55
AAA 211.03
GGG 71.07
GGA 81.16
GAA 50.97
In infants, Lewontin’s disequilibrium coefficients, D′, were 0.32 between rs737865 and val158met, 0.43 between rs737865 and rs165599, and 0.77 between val158met and rs165599, respectively. These three SNPs form seven common haplotypes with frequencies >0.03 (Table 2).

Effects of Antenatal Maternal Anxiety

Our analyses revealed no main effects of antenatal maternal anxiety on neonatal cortical thickness after adjusting for sex, birth weight adjusted by gestational age, postconceptual age on the day of MRI, and monthly household income. However, the regression analysis adjusted for sex, birth weight adjusted by gestational age, postconceptual age on the MRI day, and monthly household income revealed that the association between antenatal maternal anxiety and neonatal cortical thickness was modulated by the COMT genotypes of val158met, rs737865, and rs165599 in different anatomical regions. The interaction effect between antenatal maternal anxiety and val158met SNP was observed in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann’s areas [BAs]: 44, 45, 47; Figure 2A). The cortical thickness in these regions was negatively associated with antenatal maternal anxiety in met homozygotes (t=–2.74, df=8, p=0.03) but positively associated with antenatal maternal anxiety in val homozygotes (t=3.33, df=66, p=0.001, Figure 2B). For rs737865, the interactive effect was observed in the left precentral gyrus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BAs: 4, 6, 8; Figure 2C). The cortical thickness in these regions was negatively associated with antenatal maternal anxiety in G carriers (t=–3.61, df=44, p=0.001; Figure 2D). Furthermore, the interactive effect between antenatal maternal anxiety and rs165599 was observed in the right superior parietal cortex, precuneus, and posterior cingulate (BAs: 7 and 23; Figure 2E). The cortical thickness in these regions was negatively associated with antenatal maternal anxiety in A-allele homozygotes (t=–4.11, df=27, p<0.001) but was positively associated with antenatal maternal anxiety in the AG group of rs165599 (t=2.57, df=62, p=0.01; Figure 2F).
FIGURE 2. Statistical Maps of Interactive Effects of Antenatal Maternal Anxiety and Individual Single-Nucleotide Polymorphisms (SNPs) on Cortical Thickness in Neonatesa
a Corrected p values are shown (panels A, C, E). Scatter plots (B, D, F) illustrate the relationship of antenatal maternal anxiety and cortical thickness within each genotype of individual SNPs. Here, the thickness was computed as averaged thickness across the cortical regions highlighted on the corresponding statistical maps.
Our findings remained the same when controlling for ethnicity instead of monthly household income. Moreover, our findings presented in Figure 2 were not influenced by maternal genotype.

Modulation Effects of COMT Haplotypes

The haplotype trend regression analysis, adjusted for sex, birth weight adjusted by gestational age, postconceptual age on the day of MRI, and monthly household income (or ethnicity) revealed that associations between antenatal maternal anxiety and cortical thickness of neonates were modulated by the rs737865-val158met-rs165599 haplotypes. The A-val-G (AGG) haplotype probabilities modulated positive associations of antenatal maternal anxiety with cortical thickness in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (BAs: 44, 45, 47) and right superior parietal cortex and precuneus (BAs: 7 and 23; Figure 3A). In contrast, the G-met-A (GAA) haplotype probabilities modulated negative associations of antenatal maternal anxiety with cortical thickness in the bilateral precentral gyrus and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BAs: 4, 6, 8, 9; Figure 3B).
FIGURE 3. Statistical Maps of the Relationship Between Neonatal Cortical Thickness and Antenatal Maternal Anxiety Modulated by AGG Haplotype Probability (Panel A) and GAA Haplotype Probability (Panel B)a
a Corrected p values are shown on each panel.

Discussion

This study revealed that functional variants in COMT regulate associations between antenatal maternal anxiety and neonatal cortical morphology, particularly in the frontal and parietal regions. This effect can be characterized by allelic variation in individual SNPs (val158met, rs737865, and rs165599) and their haplotypes. Our results suggest that the association between maternal anxiety and in utero neurodevelopment is modified through complex genetic variation in COMT. Such genetic moderation may explain, in part, the variation in offspring phenotypic outcomes associated with maternal anxiety traits.
Our study first highlighted an association between antenatal maternal anxiety and right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex structure that is moderated by val158met COMT. The met homozygous infants born to mothers with higher levels of antenatal anxiety showed cortical thinning in the right ventrolateral prefrontal region, while the val homozygous infants born to mothers with similarly high levels of antenatal anxiety showed cortical thickening. These findings reveal a dramatic genotypic moderation of a common in utero environmental condition. Previous studies showed reduced gray matter volume in the lateral prefrontal cortex as a function of increased antenatal maternal anxiety (6). Heightened antenatal anxiety predicts poor cognitive control when the offspring is school age (45), as well as difficulties in executive function in adolescence (46). Moreover, rats born to mothers exposed to stress during pregnancy have significantly reduced dendritic length in pyramidal neurons of the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (47). Our findings are also consistent with those of previous studies showing that val homozygosity is associated with improved emotional regulation of the prefrontal cortex (12), decreased risk for anxiety disorders (23), and reduced anxiety-related traits (23). Val carriers show greater gray matter volume in the prefrontal cortex compared with met homozygotes in adulthood (48). Imaging studies also reveal an association between the low-activity met allele and limbic and prefrontal activation in response to aversive stimuli in healthy comparison subjects (18). Heightened emotional reactivity in met carriers may contribute to a higher risk for psychopathology (49) and thus contributes to the transgenerational transmission of vulnerability for anxiety. The met allele is less active than the val allele, resulting in a higher level of dopamine in the prefrontal cortex in val carriers (50). Antenatal maternal anxiety also predicts increased basal cortisol levels in the offspring, and glucocorticoids enhance dopamine signaling in the prefrontal cortex with accompanying disruptions to executive function (51). Rodent studies show that sustained stress-induced effects on behavior are mediated by increased dopamine signaling (52). Moreover, emotional stimuli (i.e., angry faces) activate the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (53) and, interestingly, among patients with social anxiety disorders, increased activation associates with less severe symptoms (54). Finally, activity in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex associates with treatment outcome among patients with social anxiety disorders (55), which might refer to the role of this structure in appraisal and emotional regulation in response to potential threats (56).
Interestingly, our study also revealed the role for additional COMT SNPs, notably rs737865 and rs165599, in moderating the relationship of antenatal maternal anxiety with dorsolateral prefrontal and parietal cortical thickness in neonates, independent of the val158met SNP. It is unclear why the different SNPs of the COMT gene show such apparent variable moderation effects on the associations of antenatal maternal anxiety with distinct brain regions. Nevertheless, previous studies report that variation in prefrontal function (28) is associated with risk for anxiety disorders (25, 27) and with rs737865 and rs165599 when examined together with the val158met SNP. However, other studies show that the G-allele of rs165599 is associated with prefrontal aspects of verbal memory (57), prefrontal executive function (58), and working memory (59). These findings are analogous to our findings with rs165599. G-carriers born to mothers with increased antenatal anxiety had greater thickness in the right superior parietal cortex and precuneus. The two anatomical structures receive considerable visual sensory input and are involved in episodic memory, visuospatial processing, and reflection of self and are suggested as central and well-connected hubs between parietal and prefrontal regions (60). In addition, activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, where the association with antenatal maternal anxiety was moderated by rs737865 genotype, associates with emotional regulation (61).
The COMT haplotype analysis improved statistical power for the moderating effect of COMT variants on the associations between antenatal maternal anxiety and neonatal brain morphology when compared with the single SNP analysis. The results drawn from the haplotype analysis are the combination of the findings from the analysis of individual SNPs with greater extension of the findings in the bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a region in which gray matter volume was previously shown to be associated with antenatal maternal anxiety (6). Moreover, dopamine acts at the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to regulate cognitive processing (62). Infants bearing variants associated with greater enzymatic activity (COMT 158val combined with the A-allele of rs737865 and G-allele of rs165599) and born to mothers with higher levels of antenatal anxiety showed thicker cortex in the right ventrolateral region and superior parietal lobule. In contrast, infants bearing low enzymatic activity variants (COMT 158met combined with the G-allele of rs737865 and A-allele of rs165599) and born to mothers with completely comparable high levels of antenatal anxiety had thinner cortex in the bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal regions. Dorsolateral prefrontal connectivity is associated with individual differences in behavioral inhibition (63). These findings suggest that in utero influences of antenatal maternal anxiety on the prefrontal cortex may contribute to the well-established relationship between antenatal maternal anxiety and childhood emotional function (5). These findings are thus consistent with those of a previous study showing that genetic variants linked to the risk for psychiatric illness in later life are associated with cortical structure in infants (16) and underscore the importance of the perinatal period of neurodevelopment for the study of individual differences in the risk for psychopathology (64).
The strengths of this study include a unique opportunity to examine interactive effects of genes and antenatal maternal anxiety on neural development in neonates without postnatal influences, such as postnatal parenting (6568). Second, the study of haplotypes, which characterize the linkage disequilibrium structure of several markers, is considered highly advantageous, since haplotypes may show stronger association with phenotype than individual markers because they contain more information about the underlying functional genetic variants. Using estimated haplotype probability in statistical analysis on imaging data increases statistical power and generates results unbiased to samples with uniquely determined haplotypes. A weakness of the present study is that antenatal maternal anxiety was only assessed at one time point during pregnancy. However, the mid-trimester is a critical period when neural migration and synaptogenesis of the fetal brain rapidly develop. Maternal trait anxiety tends to remain stable over the course of pregnancy (31, 32, 69). Moreover, our assessment of maternal anxiety was based on a common screening tool intended to elicit a subjective report of emotional well-being but does not constitute clinical assessment. The variations in brain structure of the offspring are thus best considered as being associated with self-reported anxious symptoms and not with clinical depression, per se. Furthermore, even though previous studies have suggested that COMT influence on brain functions varies by sex, our study did not reveal any interactive effects of COMT and sex or main effects of sex on the thickness in the brain regions presented in Figures 2 and 3 (all p values >0.1). Nevertheless, sex was entered as a covariate in all the analyses in this study. Finally, perhaps the major limitation of our study, and one common to most studies of imaging genetics, is our current lack of knowledge of the relationship between the genetic variants and region-specific variation in COMT gene expression and dopamine signaling. Nevertheless, our findings are consistent with the apparent increased level of dopamine signaling in the perinatal primate prefrontal cortex (70, 71).
In conclusion, these findings are the first, to our knowledge, to reveal that antenatal anxiety interacts with functional genotypes of COMT, resulting in alterations in the prefrontal cortical morphology of offspring. Our results suggest that an increased risk for anxiety may be transmitted from mother to child during fetal life and that the effect is dependent upon infant COMT genotype.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the GUSTO (Growing Up in Singapore Toward Health Outcomes) study group and the clinical and home visit staff, as well as the participants of this study. Members of the GUSTO study group include: Pratibha Agarwal, Arijit Biswas, Choon Looi Bong, Shirong Cai, Jerry Kok Yen Chan, Yiong Huak Chan, Cornelia Yin Ing Chee, Yin Bun Cheung, Audrey Chia, Amutha Chinnadurai, Chai Kiat Chng, Mary Foong-Fong Chong, Shang Chee Chong, Mei Chien Chua, Chun Ming Ding, Eric Andrew Finkelstein, Doris Fok, Keith M. Godfrey, Anne Eng Neo Goh, Yam Thiam Daniel Goh, Joshua J. Gooley, Wee Meng Han, Mark Hanson, Christiani Jeyakumar Henry, Joanna D. Holbrook, Chin-Ying Hsu, Hazel Inskip, Jeevesh Kapur, Ivy Yee-Man Lau, Bee Wah Lee, Yung Seng Lee, Ngee Lek, Sok Bee Lim, Yen-Ling Low, Iliana Magiati, Lourdes Mary Daniel, Cheryl Ngo, Krishnamoorthy Naiduvaje, Wei Wei Pang, Boon Long Quah, Victor Samuel Rajadurai, Mary Rauff, Salome A. Rebello, Jenny L. Richmond, Lynette Pei-Chi Shek, Allan Sheppard, Borys Shuter, Leher Singh, Shu-E Soh, Walter Stunkel, Lin Lin Su, Kok Hian Tan, Oon Hoe Teoh, Mya Thway Tint, Hugo P.S. van Bever, Rob M. van Dam, Inez Bik Yun Wong, P.C. Wong, Fabian Yap, and George Seow Heong Yeo.

References

1.
Hettema JM, Neale MC, Kendler KS: A review and meta-analysis of the genetic epidemiology of anxiety disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158:1568–1578
2.
Rice F, Jones I, Thapar A: The impact of gestational stress and prenatal growth on emotional problems in offspring: a review. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2007; 115:171–183
3.
Van den Bergh BR, Mulder EJ, Mennes M, Glover V: Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioural development of the fetus and child: links and possible mechanisms: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005; 29:237–258
4.
Teixeira JM, Fisk NM, Glover V: Association between maternal anxiety in pregnancy and increased uterine artery resistance index: cohort based study. BMJ 1999; 318:153–157
5.
Mennes M, Stiers P, Lagae L, Van den Bergh B: Long-term cognitive sequelae of antenatal maternal anxiety: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006; 30:1078–1086
6.
Buss C, Davis EP, Muftuler LT, Head K, Sandman CA: High pregnancy anxiety during mid-gestation is associated with decreased gray matter density in 6–9-year-old children. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2010; 35:141–153
7.
Huizink AC, de Medina PG, Mulder EJ, Visser GH, Buitelaar JK: Psychological measures of prenatal stress as predictors of infant temperament. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2002; 41:1078–1085
8.
Qiu A, Rifkin-Graboi A, Chen H, Chong YS, Kwek K, Gluckman PD, Fortier MV, Meaney MJ: Maternal anxiety and infants’ hippocampal development: timing matters. Transl Psychiatr 2013; 3:e306
9.
Baumann C, Klauke B, Weber H, Domschke K, Zwanzger P, Pauli P, Deckert J, Reif A: The interaction of early life experiences with COMT val158met affects anxiety sensitivity. Genes Brain Behav 2013; 12:821–829
10.
Kang HJ, Kawasawa YI, Cheng F, Zhu Y, Xu X, Li M, Sousa AM, Pletikos M, Meyer KA, Sedmak G, Guennel T, Shin Y, Johnson MB, Krsnik Z, Mayer S, Fertuzinhos S, Umlauf S, Lisgo SN, Vortmeyer A, Weinberger DR, Mane S, Hyde TM, Huttner A, Reimers M, Kleinman JE, Sestan N: Spatio-temporal transcriptome of the human brain. Nature 2011; 478:483–489
11.
Tunbridge EM, Weickert CS, Kleinman JE, Herman MM, Chen J, Kolachana BS, Harrison PJ, Weinberger DR: Catechol-O-methyltransferase enzyme activity and protein expression in human prefrontal cortex across the postnatal lifespan. Cereb Cortex 2007; 17:1206–1212
12.
Mier D, Kirsch P, Meyer-Lindenberg A: Neural substrates of pleiotropic action of genetic variation in COMT: a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry 2010; 15:918–927
13.
Desbonnet L, Tighe O, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Waddington JL, O’Tuathaigh CM: Physiological and behavioural responsivity to stress and anxiogenic stimuli in COMT-deficient mice. Behav Brain Res 2012; 228:351–358
14.
Bray NJ, Buckland PR, Williams NM, Williams HJ, Norton N, Owen MJ, O’Donovan MC: A haplotype implicated in schizophrenia susceptibility is associated with reduced COMT expression in human brain. Am J Hum Genet 2003; 73:152–161
15.
Chen J, Lipska BK, Halim N, Ma QD, Matsumoto M, Melhem S, Kolachana BS, Hyde TM, Herman MM, Apud J, Egan MF, Kleinman JE, Weinberger DR: Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein, and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am J Hum Genet 2004; 75:807–821
16.
Knickmeyer RC, Wang J, Zhu H, Geng X, Woolson S, Hamer RM, Konneker T, Lin W, Styner M, Gilmore JH: Common variants in psychiatric risk genes predict brain structure at birth. Cereb Cortex 2014; 24:1230–1246
17.
Raznahan A, Greenstein D, Lee Y, Long R, Clasen L, Gochman P, Addington A, Giedd JN, Rapoport JL, Gogtay N: Catechol-O-methyl transferase (COMT) val158met polymorphism and adolescent cortical development in patients with childhood-onset schizophrenia, their non-psychotic siblings, and healthy controls. Neuroimage 2011; 57:1517–1523
18.
Smolka MN, Schumann G, Wrase J, Grüsser SM, Flor H, Mann K, Braus DF, Goldman D, Büchel C, Heinz A: Catechol-O-methyltransferase val158met genotype affects processing of emotional stimuli in the amygdala and prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 2005; 25:836–842
19.
Massat I, Souery D, Del-Favero J, Nothen M, Blackwood D, Muir W, Kaneva R, Serretti A, Lorenzi C, Rietschel M, Milanova V, Papadimitriou GN, Dikeos D, Van Broekhoven C, Mendlewicz J: Association between COMT (Val158Met) functional polymorphism and early onset in patients with major depressive disorder in a European multicenter genetic association study. Mol Psychiatry 2005; 10:598–605
20.
Rothe C, Koszycki D, Bradwejn J, King N, Deluca V, Tharmalingam S, Macciardi F, Deckert J, Kennedy JL: Association of the Val158Met catechol-O-methyltransferase genetic polymorphism with panic disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006; 31:2237–2242
21.
McGrath M, Kawachi I, Ascherio A, Colditz GA, Hunter DJ, De Vivo I: Association between catechol-O-methyltransferase and phobic anxiety. Am J Psychiatry 2004; 161:1703–1705
22.
Sheikh HI, Kryski KR, Smith HJ, Dougherty LR, Klein DN, Bufferd SJ, Singh SM, Hayden EP: Catechol-O-methyltransferase gene val158met polymorphism and depressive symptoms during early childhood. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2013; 162B:245–252
23.
Woo JM, Yoon KS, Choi YH, Oh KS, Lee YS, Yu BH: The association between panic disorder and the L/L genotype of catechol-O-methyltransferase. J Psychiatr Res 2004; 38:365–370
24.
Domschke K, Deckert J, O’donovan MC, Glatt SJ: Meta-analysis of COMT val158met in panic disorder: ethnic heterogeneity and gender specificity. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2007; 144B:667–673
25.
Hettema JM, An SS, Bukszar J, van den Oord EJ, Neale MC, Kendler KS, Chen X: Catechol-O-methyltransferase contributes to genetic susceptibility shared among anxiety spectrum phenotypes. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 64:302–310
26.
Stein MB, Fallin MD, Schork NJ, Gelernter J: COMT polymorphisms and anxiety-related personality traits. Neuropsychopharmacology 2005; 30:2092–2102
27.
Funke B, Malhotra AK, Finn CT, Plocik AM, Lake SL, Lencz T, DeRosse P, Kane JM, Kucherlapati R: COMT genetic variation confers risk for psychotic and affective disorders: a case control study. Behav Brain Funct 2005; 1:19
28.
Meyer-Lindenberg A, Nichols T, Callicott JH, Ding J, Kolachana B, Buckholtz J, Mattay VS, Egan M, Weinberger DR: Impact of complex genetic variation in COMT on human brain function. Mol Psychiatry 2006; 11:867–877
29.
Stephens M, Donnelly P: A comparison of bayesian methods for haplotype reconstruction from population genotype data. Am J Hum Genet 2003; 73:1162–1169
30.
Meades R, Ayers S: Anxiety measures validated in perinatal populations: a systematic review. J Affect Disord 2011; 133:1–15
31.
Roesch SC, Schetter CD, Woo G, Hobel CJ: Modeling the types and timing of stress in pregnancy. Anxiety Stress Coping 2004; 17:87–102
32.
Dipietro JA, Costigan KA, Sipsma HL: Continuity in self-report measures of maternal anxiety, stress, and depressive symptoms from pregnancy through two years postpartum. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol 2008; 29:115–124
33.
Smith SM: Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum Brain Mapp 2002; 17:143–155
34.
Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, van der Kouwe A, Killiany R, Kennedy D, Klaveness S, Montillo A, Makris N, Rosen B, Dale AM: Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 2002; 33:341–355
35.
Han X, Xu C, Braga-Neto U, Prince JL: Topology correction in brain cortex segmentation using a multiscale, graph-based algorithm. IEEE Trans Med Imaging 2002; 21:109–121
36.
Zhong J, Qiu A: Multi-manifold diffeomorphic metric mapping for aligning cortical hemispheric surfaces. Neuroimage 2010; 49:355–365
37.
Bai J, Abdul-Rahman MF, Rifkin-Graboi A, Chong YS, Kwek K, Saw SM, Godfrey KM, Gluckman PD, Fortier MV, Meaney MJ, Qiu A: Population differences in brain morphology and microstructure among Chinese, Malay, and Indian neonates. PLoS ONE 2012; 7:e47816
38.
Joshi S, Davis B, Jomier M, Gerig G: Unbiased diffeomorphic atlas construction for computational anatomy. Neuroimage 2004; 23(suppl 1):S151–S160
39.
Jiang L, Willner D, Danoy P, Xu H, Brown MA: Comparison of the performance of two commercial genome-wide association study genotyping platforms in Han Chinese samples. G3 (Bethesda) 2013; 3:23–29
40.
Oliphant A, Barker DL, Stuelpnagel JR, Chee MS: BeadArray technology: enabling an accurate, cost-effective approach to high-throughput genotyping. Biotechniques 2002; (suppl):56–58, 60–61
41.
Barrett JC, Fry B, Maller J, Daly MJ: Haploview: analysis and visualization of LD and haplotype maps. Bioinformatics 2005; 21:263–265
42.
Chung MK, Robbins SM, Dalton KM, Davidson RJ, Alexander AL, Evans AC: Cortical thickness analysis in autism with heat kernel smoothing. Neuroimage 2005; 25:1256–1265
43.
Chung MK, Worsley KJ, Nacewicz BM, Dalton KM, Davidson RJ: General multivariate linear modeling of surface shapes using SurfStat. Neuroimage 2010; 53:491–505
44.
Zaykin DV, Westfall PH, Young SS, Karnoub MA, Wagner MJ, Ehm MG: Testing association of statistically inferred haplotypes with discrete and continuous traits in samples of unrelated individuals. Hum Hered 2002; 53:79–91
45.
Loomans EM, van der Stelt O, van Eijsden M, Gemke RJ, Vrijkotte TG, Van den Bergh BR: High levels of antenatal maternal anxiety are associated with altered cognitive control in five-year-old children. Dev Psychobiol 2012; 54:441–450
46.
Van den Bergh BR, Mennes M, Oosterlaan J, Stevens V, Stiers P, Marcoen A, Lagae L: High antenatal maternal anxiety is related to impulsivity during performance on cognitive tasks in 14- and 15-year-olds. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005; 29:259–269
47.
Murmu MS, Salomon S, Biala Y, Weinstock M, Braun K, Bock J: Changes of spine density and dendritic complexity in the prefrontal cortex in offspring of mothers exposed to stress during pregnancy. Eur J Neurosci 2006; 24:1477–1487
48.
Rowe JB, Hughes L, Williams-Gray CH, Bishop S, Fallon S, Barker RA, Owen AM: The val158met COMT polymorphism’s effect on atrophy in healthy aging and Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2010; 31:1064–1068
49.
Collip D, van Winkel R, Peerbooms O, Lataster T, Thewissen V, Lardinois M, Drukker M, Rutten BP, Van Os J, Myin-Germeys I: COMT Val158Met-stress interaction in psychosis: role of background psychosis risk. CNS Neurosci Ther 2011; 17:612–619
50.
Goldberg TE, Egan MF, Gscheidle T, Coppola R, Weickert T, Kolachana BS, Goldman D, Weinberger DR: Executive subprocesses in working memory: relationship to catechol-O-methyltransferase Val158Met genotype and schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003; 60:889–896
51.
Butts KA, Weinberg J, Young AH, Phillips AG: Glucocorticoid receptors in the prefrontal cortex regulate stress-evoked dopamine efflux and aspects of executive function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011; 108:18459–18464
52.
Nestler EJ, Carlezon WA Jr: The mesolimbic dopamine reward circuit in depression. Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59:1151–1159
53.
Stein MB, Goldin PR, Sareen J, Zorrilla LT, Brown GG: Increased amygdala activation to angry and contemptuous faces in generalized social phobia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59:1027–1034
54.
Monk CS, Nelson EE, McClure EB, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Leibenluft E, Blair RJ, Chen G, Charney DS, Ernst M, Pine DS: Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation and attentional bias in response to angry faces in adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163:1091–1097
55.
Doehrmann O, Ghosh SS, Polli FE, Reynolds GO, Horn F, Keshavan A, Triantafyllou C, Saygin ZM, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Hofmann SG, Pollack M, Gabrieli JD: Predicting treatment response in social anxiety disorder from functional magnetic resonance imaging. JAMA Psychiatry 2013; 70:87–97
56.
Goldin PR, Manber T, Hakimi S, Canli T, Gross JJ: Neural bases of social anxiety disorder: emotional reactivity and cognitive regulation during social and physical threat. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009; 66:170–180
57.
Burdick KE, Funke B, Goldberg JF, Bates JA, Jaeger J, Kucherlapati R, Malhotra AK: COMT genotype increases risk for bipolar I disorder and influences neurocognitive performance. Bipolar Disord 2007; 9:370–376
58.
Chan RC, Chen RY, Chen EY, Hui TC, Cheung EF, Cheung HK, Sham P, Li T, Collier D: The differential clinical and neurocognitive profiles of COMT SNP rs165599 genotypes in schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2005; 11:202–204
59.
Barnett JH, Heron J, Goldman D, Jones PB, Xu K: Effects of catechol-O-methyltransferase on normal variation in the cognitive function of children. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166:909–916
60.
Bullmore E, Sporns O: Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009; 10:186–198
61.
Golkar A, Lonsdorf TB, Olsson A, Lindstrom KM, Berrebi J, Fransson P, Schalling M, Ingvar M, Öhman A: Distinct contributions of the dorsolateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex during emotion regulation. PLoS ONE 2012; 7:e48107
62.
Goldman-Rakic PS, Castner SA, Svensson TH, Siever LJ, Williams GV: Targeting the dopamine D1 receptor in schizophrenia: insights for cognitive dysfunction. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004; 174:3–16
63.
Hardee JE, Benson BE, Bar-Haim Y, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Chen G, Britton JC, Ernst M, Fox NA, Pine DS, Pérez-Edgar K: Patterns of neural connectivity during an attention bias task moderate associations between early childhood temperament and internalizing symptoms in young adulthood. Biol Psychiatry 2013; 74:273–279
64.
Gilmore JH, Kang C, Evans DD, Wolfe HM, Smith JK, Lieberman JA, Lin W, Hamer RM, Styner M, Gerig G: Prenatal and neonatal brain structure and white matter maturation in children at high risk for schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:1083–1091
65.
Peterson BS, Weissman MM: A brain-based endophenotype for major depressive disorder. Annu Rev Med 2011; 62:461–474
66.
Chen MC, Hamilton JP, Gotlib IH: Decreased hippocampal volume in healthy girls at risk of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2010; 67:270–276
67.
Field T, Diego M, Dieter J, Hernandez-Reif M, Schanberg S, Kuhn C, Yando R, Bendell D: Prenatal depression effects on the fetus and the newborn. Infant Behav Dev 2004; 27:216–229
68.
Fleming AS, Flett GL, Ruble DN, Shaul DL: Postpartum adjustment in 1st-time mothers: relations between mood, maternal attitudes, and mother infant interactions. Dev Psychol 1988; 24:71–81
69.
Heron J, O’Connor TG, Evans J, Golding J, Glover V; ALSPAC Study Team: The course of anxiety and depression through pregnancy and the postpartum in a community sample. J Affect Disord 2004; 80:65–73
70.
Goldman-Rakic PS, Brown RM: Postnatal development of monoamine content and synthesis in the cerebral cortex of rhesus monkeys. Brain Res 1982; 256:339–349
71.
Weickert CS, Webster MJ, Gondipalli P, Rothmond D, Fatula RJ, Herman MM, Kleinman JE, Akil M: Postnatal alterations in dopaminergic markers in the human prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience 2007; 144:1109–1119

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 163 - 172
PubMed: 25320962

History

Received: 10 March 2014
Revision received: 21 May 2014
Revision received: 8 July 2014
Accepted: 18 July 2014
Published ahead of print: 31 October 2014
Published online: 1 February 2015
Published in print: February 01, 2015

Authors

Details

Anqi Qiu, Ph.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Ta Anh Tuan, M.S.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Mei Lyn Ong, Ph.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Yue Li, B.S.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Helen Chen, M.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Anne Rifkin-Graboi, Ph.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Birit F.P. Broekman, M.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Kenneth Kwek, M.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Seang-Mei Saw, Ph.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Yap-Seng Chong, M.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Peter D. Gluckman, Ph.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Marielle V. Fortier, M.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Joanna Dawn Holbrook, Ph.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.
Michael J. Meaney, Ph.D.
From the Department of Biomedical Engineering and Clinical Imaging Research Center, National University of Singapore, Singapore; the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore; the Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Imaging, KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; the Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal; the Sackler Program for Epigenetics and Psychobiology at McGill University, Montreal; the Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, National University Health System, Singapore; the KK Women’s and Children’s Hospital, Singapore; and the Saw Swee Hock School of Public Health, National University of Singapore, Singapore.

Notes

Address correspondence to Dr. Qiu ([email protected]).

Funding Information

Dr. Chong has received speaker’s fees from companies selling nutritional products and that are part of an academic consortium that has received research funding from Abbott Nutrition, Danone Nutrition, and Nestle. Dr. Gluckman has received travel expenses to speak at meetings sponsored by Abbott Nutrition, Danone Nutrition, and Nestle and has served as part of an academic consortium that has received research support from these companies unrelated to this study. All other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.Supported by the National Medical Research Council (NMRC; NMRC/TCR/004-NUS/2008 and NMRC/CBRG/0039/2013), the Young Investigator Award at the National University of Singapore (NUSYIA FY10 P07), the National University of Singapore MOE AcRF Tier 1, and the Singapore Ministry of Education Academic Research Fund Tier 2 (MOE2012-T2-2-130).

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share