Skip to main content

Abstract

Objective:

Childhood irritability is a common, impairing problem with changing age-related manifestations that predict long-term adverse outcomes. However, more investigation of overall and age-specific neural correlates is needed. Because youths with irritability exhibit exaggerated responses to frustrating stimuli, the authors used a frustration functional MRI (fMRI) paradigm to examine associations between irritability and neural activation and tested the moderating effect of age.

Method:

The authors studied a transdiagnostic sample of 195 youths with varying levels of irritability (disruptive mood dysregulation disorder, N=52; anxiety disorder, N=42; attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, N=40; and healthy volunteers, N=61). Irritability was measured by parent and child reports on the Affective Reactivity Index. The fMRI paradigm was a cued-attention task differentiating neural activity in response to frustration (rigged feedback) from activity during attention orienting in the trial following frustration.

Results:

Whole-brain activation analyses revealed associations with irritability during attention orienting following frustration. Irritability was positively associated with frontal-striatal activation, specifically in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, and caudate. Age moderated the association between irritability and activation in some frontal and posterior regions (the anterior cingulate cortex, medial frontal gyrus, cuneus, precuneus, and superior parietal lobule [F=19.04–28.51, df=1, 189, partial eta squared=0.09–0.13]). Specifically, higher irritability was more strongly related to increased activation in younger youths compared with older youths.

Conclusions:

Following frustration, levels of irritability correlated with activity in neural systems mediating attention orienting, top-down regulation of emotions, and motor execution. Although most associations were independent of age, dysfunction in the anterior cingulate cortex and posterior regions was more pronounced in young children with irritability.
Irritability can be defined as an increased propensity to experience anger and frustration, compared with peers (1). It is a serious and common mental health problem among youths (1, 2). It predicts adult depressive and anxiety disorders (a genetically mediated association) as well as long-term impairment (e.g., high suicidality and low educational and income attainment) (13). Irritability is also a core feature of the new DSM-5 category of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder, which is characterized by developmentally inappropriate, frequent, and severe temper outbursts (2, 4). Youths with severe irritability experience significant impairment in multiple domains (e.g., at home, in school, and with peers) and have high rates of service use, hospitalization, and school suspensions (2, 4). However, there are few evidence-based treatments for irritability (5). A better understanding of the pathophysiology of irritability is essential to guide the development of novel mechanism-based treatments for this common and impairing problem.
Given the increased proneness to frustration associated with irritability, and working from a translational neuroscience perspective, the neural mechanisms mediating irritability can be captured by studying an organism’s neural responses to frustrative nonreward (1, 2). Originally operationalized in rodents, frustrative nonreward is the psychological state induced by the failure to receive a reward that an organism has been conditioned to expect (6). In humans, investigators can model frustrative nonreward during functional MRI (fMRI) by evoking frustration in real time while assessing its neural correlates (2). In a large, transdiagnostic sample, we used such methods to examine associations between irritability and neural activation during an fMRI paradigm that models frustrative nonreward.
Our second goal was to examine age-related variation in the association between irritability and neural activity. Normative responses to frustration change during development (7), yet little research has examined the underlying neural mechanisms (8). Indeed, while many behavioral studies document the development of emotion regulation (9, 10), neuroimaging research has just begun to elucidate the maturation of brain systems supporting such capacity (11, 12). For example, the protracted development of the prefrontal cortex has been linked to age-related improvements in attention shifting and cognitive control, which can modulate affective arousal (1114). Although age-related brain mechanisms have been examined in some clinical populations (e.g., in persons with anxiety or depression) (15, 16), little work has been done on irritability (8, 17, 18).
Few studies utilize frustration paradigms in youths with irritability, and those that do report neural dysfunction in the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex, striatum, amygdala, and parietal cortex (1922). However, the sample sizes of these studies are insufficiently large to generate clear conclusions (23). To address this issue, we recruited a relatively large sample of 195 youths. Moreover, no previous fMRI research, to our knowledge, has dissociated neural responses to a frustrating event from the impact of frustration on the neural mechanisms mediating performance on a subsequent cognitive trial. Here, we examined the neural response to frustration and the impact of frustration on attention orienting. We defined the latter as the ability to disengage attention from the current focus, move attention to a selected alternative target, and direct attention to that target (24). We chose an attention-orienting task that engages frontal systems (e.g., the ventral attention network) known to mediate this process (14) and adapted a frustrating task used previously (19) to track adjustments in this brain system following frustration. The task induces frustration by falsely informing study subjects that they have committed errors. Given the critical role of the executive attention system in adapting to errors (14), this system may thus be important in the context of frustration. It is also noteworthy that recent work has implicated attention orienting (in response to threat) as a potential mechanism of irritability (1, 2, 25).
We examined associations between irritability, measured dimensionally, and neural activations during a frustrating attention-orienting task. Specifically, we studied a transdiagnostic clinical sample of 195 youths with varying levels of irritability. Based on previous work (1922), we hypothesized that irritability would be associated with perturbed activation in frontal-striatal-amygdalar regions during feedback processing and during attention orienting following frustrating feedback. Additionally, given the protracted nature of prefrontal cortex development (12, 13), we hypothesized that associations between irritability and activation in this region would be moderated by age.

Method

Participants

The study sample included 195 children and adolescents ages 8–18 (mean age, 12.9 years [SD=2.3]) with well-distributed irritability levels and a mean above the cutoff for severe irritability (26). Participants had primary diagnoses of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (characterized by severe, chronic irritability) (N=52), anxiety disorder (N=42), attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (N=40), or no disorder (N=61) (Table 1) (for further details, see Tables S1 and S2 in the online supplement). They were recruited from National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) clinics between February 2012 and July 2016. On the Children’s Global Assessment Scale (27), 58% of the sample had a score ≤60, indicating at least “some noticeable problems” in several areas. Most were seeking or receiving treatment; 48% were being treated with medication, and half of these were taking two or more types of medications. Some patients with disruptive mood dysregulation disorder were in an inpatient treatment trial. Patients with anxiety disorder were in an outpatient treatment trial. This study was approved by the NIMH institutional review board, and written consent and assent, respectively, were obtained from parents and children. Exclusion criteria included an IQ <70, pervasive developmental or neurological disorders, substance abuse within the past 2 months, and lifetime history of psychosis, conduct disorder, or unstable or chronic medical illness. For detailed diagnostic and clinical assessments and additional inclusion and exclusion criteria for the anxiety disorder group, see the Methods section in the online supplement.
TABLE 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of the Study Sample
Characteristic  
 MeanSD
Age (years)12.872.35
IQa111.8213.22
Socioeconomic statusb32.0317.07
Motionc0.120.07
Children’s Global Assessment Scale (past 6 months)d57.5812.39
Dimensional measures  
 Affective Reactivity Index3.072.68
 Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders16.5511.40
 Conners’ Parent Rating Scale59.4814.05
 N%
Male9850.30
Primary diagnosis  
 Disruptive mood dysregulation disorder5226.67
 Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder4020.51
 Anxiety4221.54
 No diagnosis6131.28
Medications  
 Stimulants4523.08
 Antidepressants3115.90
 Antipsychotics105.13
a
Measured with the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; data are missing for one participant.
b
Measured with the Hollingshead Two-Factor Index of Socioeconomic Status; data are missing for 25 participants.
c
Calculated as the mean Euclidean distance of framewise volume shift after censoring.
d
Data were collected for patients only (i.e., children and youths with a diagnosis of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder, anxiety disorder, or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder); data are missing for 13 patients.

Measures

We assessed irritability using parent and child reports from the Affective Reactivity Index (ARI) (28). To control for co-occurring anxiety and ADHD symptoms, we collected parent and child reports from the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED) rating scale (29) and parent reports from the Conners’ Parent Rating Scale–Revised (CPRS-R) long form (30). Total scores from child and parent reports were averaged for the ARI and SCARED. The ADHD index T score from the CPRS-R was used to index ADHD symptoms. For participant distribution (by diagnosis) along these three symptom dimensions, see Figure S1 in the online supplement. For more than 90% of the sample, these measures were collected within 3 months of scanning. The 3-month window was selected on the basis of the stability of the measures and to maximize the sample size. These measures have been shown to be highly stable across periods longer than 3 months (e.g., see Stringaris et al. [28]; see also the Methods section in the online supplement).

fMRI Paradigm

Participants completed a frustrating attention task, the affective Posner 2 paradigm (31), which was adapted from a previous fMRI study (19) and demonstrates good reliability and validity (31). Participants were asked to identify a target following a cue by button press (left or right). The target appeared in the same location as the cue (valid trials) for 75% of trials and in the opposite location (invalid trials) for 25%. The task consisted of two non-frustration runs during which participants received accurate or positive feedback and two frustration runs during which they received rigged or positive feedback (60% and 40% of correct trials, respectively). Each run lasted 8 minutes (Figure 1) (see also Figure S2 in the online supplement). Imaging analyses focused on the two frustration runs. Group-level analyses were conducted separately for the feedback and attention portions of the task, which were separated by jitter (1,000–3,000 ms, with an average of 2,000 ms) (Figure 1). The feedback portion (Figure 1) probed neural activity during processing of rigged feedback compared with positive feedback. The attention portion (“N+1” trial) (Figure 1) assessed neural activity during the attentional event following rigged feedback compared with positive feedback. At the end of each run, participants rated their feelings of unhappiness and frustration on 9-point Likert scales. For further details regarding task procedures, see the Methods section in the online supplement.
FIGURE 1. Trial Structure During Frustration Runs of the Affective Posner 2 Taska
a In frustration runs, 60% of correct responses were followed by rigged feedback (“TOO SLOW!”), and 40% of correct responses were followed by positive feedback (“YOU WIN!”). All incorrect responses were followed by negative feedback (“WRONG!”). Imaging analyses focused on the N+1 trial (red square) and the “feedback” (blue square) portions of the task. Neural responses for the N+1 trial were modeled from the onset of the two boxes for 2 seconds; neural responses for the feedback portion were modeled for the whole duration of the feedback stimulus (2,000 ms). Significant associations with irritability emerged from the N+1 trial (i.e., the attentional event following rigged feedback compared with positive feedback [red square]). ISI=interstimulus interval; ITI=intertrial interval.

Imaging Acquisition

Neuroimaging data were acquired on a 3-T General Electric scanner (General Electric, Boston) using an eight-channel head coil. A high-resolution anatomical scan (1-mm slices, three-dimensional spoiled gradient-echo sequence, 7° flip angle, minimum full echo time, 256×256 matrix, 25.6-cm field of view) and gradient echo-planar imaging images were collected (repetition time=2,300 ms, echo time=25 ms, 24-cm field of view, voxel size=2.5×2.5×3 mm, 206 volumes per run, flip angle=75° [N=134] or 90° [N=61]). For further details, see the Methods section in the online supplement.

Imaging Preprocessing and Individual Analysis

Data were analyzed using Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI). Preprocessing included despiking, temporal alignment to the first acquired slice, coregistration, smoothing, masking, and intensity scaling. Repetition time pairs with a Euclidean norm motion derivative >1 mm were censored during linear regression. A general linear model estimated voxel-wise blood-oxygen-level-dependent signal change (see the Methods section in the online supplement).

Data analyses.

Behavioral and post hoc imaging analyses were conducted with SPSS (IBM, Armonk, N.Y.). For behavioral results (frustration and unhappiness ratings, accuracy, and reaction time), see the Results section in the online supplement. Group-level whole-brain activation analyses were conducted using AFNI’s 3dMVM, a multivariate model-based analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) appropriate for our fMRI data and study design (32). Analyses examined the effects of ARI, age, and the ARI-by-age interaction, with SCARED score, CPRS-R score, and motion (for imaging data) as covariates. All variables were continuous, and they were mean centered to reduce multicollinearity due to testing of the interaction term (ARI by age). Correlations between dimensional measures (rs <0.54) had an acceptable tolerance in the model with ARI, age, ARI-by-age interaction, SCARED score, CPRS-R score, and motion (variance inflation factors ≤1.46) (for further details, see Table S2 in the online supplement).

Imaging data.

Imaging analyses focused on frustration runs because only these runs contained both positive and rigged feedback. We did not directly compare non-frustration and frustration runs (e.g., on positive feedback) because there was a fundamental difference in “baselines”: the baselines in frustration runs were likely to be more saturated and elevated. However, we conducted a whole-brain analysis for non-frustration runs only and, compared with the baseline, found no significant associations between irritability and neural activation during either positive feedback or attention orienting following positive feedback.
Only valid, correct trials were included, given the insufficient numbers of other trial types. Separate analyses were conducted for the feedback portion of the N trial and the attentional portion of the N+1 trial (Figure 1). For the feedback portion of the N trial, an ARI-by-age-by-condition (rigged compared with positive feedback) ANCOVA was conducted to assess activation during processing of rigged feedback compared with positive feedback and how it varied with irritability and age. For the N+1 trial, an ARI-by-age-by-condition (after rigged compared with positive feedback) ANCOVA was conducted to assess activation during the attentional event following rigged feedback compared with positive feedback and how it varied with irritability and age.
A whole-brain gray matter mask was used in the analyses, including voxels for which data existed for ≥90% of participants, voxel-wise p values of 0.001, and multiple-testing correction alphas of 0.05 via Monte Carlo cluster-size simulation. By using methods designed recently to address concerns regarding inflated false positive rates (33), the cluster size surviving whole-brain correction was set at 703 mm3. At this threshold, we observed a large cluster of 68,000 mm3 for the three-way ARI-by-age-by-condition interaction and a cluster of 86,281 mm3 for the two-way ARI-by-condition interaction from the N+1 analyses. To facilitate interpretation, we extracted clusters by using a more stringent voxel-wise p value of 0.0001 (clusters ≥203 mm3) (Table 2). Using this threshold, we created a conjunction map of the significant three-way and two-way interactions and calculated the shared voxels (i.e., voxels that showed significant effects for both the three-way ARI-by-age-by-condition interaction and the two-way ARI-by-condition interaction). Only 8.4% of the voxels were shared, suggesting that most voxels that showed a significant ARI-by-condition interaction were not moderated by age. To deconstruct significant interactions, mean activation across voxels in significant clusters were extracted using AFNI’s 3dROIstat for follow-up analyses in SPSS. For the N trial analyses, no significant clusters were observed (i.e., ARI, age, and their interaction were not associated with activation during processing of rigged feedback compared with positive feedback).
TABLE 2. N+1 Trial: Effect of Affective Reactivity Index (ARI)-by-Age-by-Condition and ARI-by-Condition Interactions From Whole-Brain Activation Analysisa
   Analysisd 
RegionsbSize (mm3)Peak Coordinates (x, y, z)cFpηp2Correlation (r)e
ARI-by-age-by-condition interaction      
 Right cuneus2,5639, –79, 1625.25<0.0010.12 
 Right superior parietal lobule2,56331, –64, 4428.51<0.0010.13 
 Left precuneus and cuneus1,469–24, –69, 2422.91<0.0010.11 
 Left medial frontal gyrus and anterior cingulate cortex1,469–9, 44, 2623.94<0.0010.11 
 Right pre- and post-central gyri1,35939, –19, 4925.62<0.0010.12 
 Left precuneus1,047–14, –66, 4125.15<0.0010.12 
 Left middle frontal gyrus641–34, 11, 4120.80<0.0010.10 
 Right middle occipital gyrus62529, –84, 2125.18<0.0010.12 
 Right postcentral gyrus50019, –29, 6622.25<0.0010.11 
 Right superior temporal gyrus46951, –51, 2120.33<0.0010.10 
 Right superior frontal gyrus43811, 54, 2921.70<0.0010.10 
 Right lingual and fusiform gyrus42221, –61, –421.18<0.0010.10 
 Left precentral gyrus391–34, –9, 4421.34<0.0010.10 
 Right middle and superior frontal gyrus37524, 24, 4119.23<0.0010.09 
 Right middle frontal gyrus31324, –6, 4419.75<0.0010.10 
 Right fusiform gyrus29736, –59, –1419.41<0.0010.09 
 Left superior parietal lobule281–29, –61, 4420.58<0.0010.10 
 Left medial frontal gyrus234–6, –11, 5119.04<0.0010.09 
 Left superior frontal gyrus203–9, 14, 5420.03<0.0010.10 
ARI-by-condition interaction      
 Left and right cingulate gyrus, right superior frontal gyrus13,5949, 19, 4134.81<0.0010.160.40
 Right middle frontal gyrus5,84436, 16, 4131.63<0.0010.140.38
 Left middle frontal gyrus2,469–31, 21, 3425.85<0.0010.120.35
 Right caudate, thalamus2,42211, –19, 1927.72<0.0010.130.36
 Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex1,42246, 31, 2629.23<0.0010.140.37
 Right cuneus1,2509, –76, 1923.65<0.0010.110.33
 Right precuneus1,09419, –69, 4125.68<0.0010.120.35
 Left middle frontal gyrus1,047–34, –1, 4622.33<0.0010.110.33
 Right inferior frontal gyrus59454, 26, 628.29<0.0010.130.36
 Left pre- and post-central gyri531–39, –19, 3920.81<0.0010.100.32
 Left parahippocampal gyrus469–16, –39, –423.86<0.0010.110.34
 Left caudate453–9, 6, 2121.53<0.0010.100.32
 Right superior temporal gyrus40641, –49, 1921.37<0.0010.100.32
 Right precentral gyrus34456, 6, 621.29<0.0010.100.32
 Left precentral gyrus266–61, –1, 1420.85<0.0010.100.32
 Left cingulate gyrus203–6, –16, 4120.77<0.0010.100.32
 Left superior frontal gyrus203–6, 11, 5919.68<0.0010.100.31
a
The condition effect refers to the attention portion of the trial immediately after receiving rigged compared with positive feedback (i.e., the N+1 trial).
b
The region comprising the greatest portion of the cluster extent is presented. At a voxel-wise p value of 0.001, the largest significant cluster was 86,281 mm3. To facilitate interpretation, we extracted clusters using the more stringent voxel-wise p value of 0.0001. At this threshold, clusters ≥203 mm3 survived whole-brain correction at an alpha of 0.05.
c
Coordinates are in Talairach space.
d
Post hoc analysis of covariance on the mean blood-oxygen-level-dependent signal for extracted clusters are presented. The df value for all F statistics is 1, 188.
e
Correlations between ARI and the difference in brain activation after receiving rigged compared with positive feedback (rigged minus positive) are presented, after adjusting for motion and symptoms of anxiety and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. It is noteworthy that these correlations may be inflated given that they were computed on the basis of extracted signal change from voxels that survived whole-brain correction (for further details, see Vul et al. [43]).
To address concerns regarding threshold-based cluster forming and replicability of imaging findings, we reanalyzed our data using the threshold-free cluster enhancement approach (34), with a family-wise error rate correction level of 0.05, by permutation testing (for further details, see the Results section and Figure S9 in the online supplement). Results were largely consistent with the original analysis in AFNI, except that two small clusters for the ARI-by-condition effect (in the left caudate and precentral gyrus) became nonsignificant (the right caudate remained significant).

Results

Imaging Data

Significant findings emerged from the N+1 analysis, in the comparison of neural activity occurring after rigged feedback compared with positive feedback during the frustration runs (Figure 1). Specifically, we report interactions involving condition (after rigged compared with positive feedback) and ARI (dimensional measure of irritability) (i.e., ARI-by-age-by-condition and ARI-by-condition).

ARI-by-age-by-condition.

During the attentional event immediately following rigged feedback compared with positive feedback, activation in several posterior and frontal regions varied with levels of irritability and as a function of participant age. These regions include the cuneus, precuneus, superior parietal lobule, medial frontal gyrus, and anterior cingulate cortex (Table 2, Figure 2; see also Figure S3 in the online supplement). Across these regions, higher irritability was more strongly related to increased activation in younger compared with older youths. Specifically, higher irritability was significantly related to increased activation in young children (ages 8–11.5) and younger adolescents (ages 11.5–14), with the strongest correlation in young children. However, irritability was not related to activation in older adolescents (ages 14–18). Findings in the medial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex are summarized in Figure 2.
FIGURE 2. Age Moderating the Association Between Irritability and Activation in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Anterior Cingulate Cortex During Attention Orienting Following Rigged Compared With Positive Feedbacka
a Panel A shows the left medial prefrontal cortex extending to the anterior cingulate cortex from the whole-brain N+1 trial activation analysis. During the attentional portion of the trial, activation after receiving rigged compared with positive feedback varied with irritability (i.e., Affective Reactivity Index [ARI] scores) and age. Panel B shows the associations among ARI, age, and brain activation (all variables were continuous). The blood-oxygen-level-dependent percent signal change for this cluster was extracted for each condition (the N+1 trial after rigged feedback and after positive feedback) for each study subject. These values were entered in the same analysis of covariance model (controlling for symptoms of anxiety and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [ADHD] and motion) as in the main analysis, and predicted percent signal changes were generated. The differences between the predicted percent signal changes after rigged compared with positive feedback are plotted. The three-dimensional graph shows that after receiving rigged compared with positive feedback, younger youths with high irritability exhibited increased activation. Panel C shows partial regression plots (controlling for symptoms of anxiety and ADHD and motion) by age tertiles (for each age group, N=65) depicting individual data points and the association between ARI (mean-centered) and the percent signal change difference between trials occurring after rigged feedback compared with positive feedback. Age was treated as a continuous variable in the analyses. The age tertiles here are for visualization purposes only. Higher irritability was more strongly related to increased activation on this contrast in early childhood (ages 8–11.5) compared with early adolescence (ages 11.5–14). Irritability was not related to activation in late adolescence (ages 14–18). It is noteworthy that these correlations may be inflated given that they were computed on the basis of extracted signal change from voxels that survived whole-brain correction (43). F=F value; L=left; R=right.

ARI-by-condition.

This two-way interaction yielded significant findings in the left and right cingulate gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, caudate, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, cuneus, precuneus, and inferior frontal gyrus (Table 2, Figure 3; see also Figure S4 in the online supplement). Most (91.6%) significant voxels in these regions did not overlap with the significant voxels for the three-way interaction described above. The few overlapping regions included the left and right cingulate gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, cuneus, and precuneus. Given that these regions were qualified by a three-way interaction of ARI by age by condition, we do not interpret them here. Regions showing a significant ARI-by-condition effect without a qualifying three-way interaction included the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Figure 3), caudate (extending to the thalamus), and inferior frontal gyrus, among others (Table 2; see also Figure S4 in the online supplement). Across these regions, higher irritability was related to increased activation following rigged feedback compared with positive feedback.
FIGURE 3. Association Between Irritability and Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Activation During Attention Orienting Following Rigged Compared With Positive Feedbacka
a Panel A shows the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex for the whole-brain N+1 trial activation analysis. During the attentional portion of the trial, activation after receiving rigged compared with positive feedback varied with irritability (i.e., Affective Reactivity Index [ARI] scores). Panel B shows partial regression plots (controlling for symptoms of anxiety and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and motion) depicting individual data points and the association between ARI (mean-centered) and the percent signal change difference between trials occurring after rigged compared with positive feedback. Higher irritability was related to more activation on this contrast. It is noteworthy that these correlations may be inflated given that they were computed on the basis of extracted signal change from voxels that survived whole-brain correction (for further details, see Vul et al. [43]). F=F value; L=left; R=right.

Post Hoc Analyses: Depressive Symptoms, Frustration and Unhappiness Ratings, Medication, DSM-5 Diagnosis, and Gender

Given the longitudinal link between childhood irritability and depression later in life (3), we evaluated the effect of depression by conducting whole-brain analyses with depressive symptoms (measured by the self-rated Children’s Depression Inventory [35]) as the main dimension. There were no significant associations between depressive symptoms and neural activations (threshold voxel-wise p=0.001, cluster extent ≥703 mm3). Depressive symptoms were evaluated this way and were not treated as a covariate in the main analyses (unlike anxiety and ADHD symptoms) because 28 participants had missing data.
We also conducted analyses to examine the effects of “state irritability” measured by self-ratings of frustration and unhappiness obtained at the end of each of the two frustration runs (see the Results section in the online supplement). The only significant finding was a positive association between unhappiness and activation in the right superior temporal gyrus during processing of rigged feedback compared with positive feedback. These analyses have limitations because of the problems associated with the state measures (see the Results section in the online supplement). Nonetheless, they suggest that the neural substrates underlying transient, subjective feelings of frustration and unhappiness may differ from the neural substrates mediating trait irritability.
We evaluated the confounding effect of medications by iteratively excluding participants by medication class (stimulants, antidepressants, and antipsychotics) (for further details, see Tables S3 and S4 in the online supplement). All significant findings remained. Additionally, analyses comparing medicated (N=25) with nonmedicated (N=100) participants on the day of the scan did not reveal significant between-group differences on the imaging findings.
Categorical analyses using diagnoses instead of symptom dimensions yielded null results in whole-brain activation but a few findings in functional connectivity (for further details, see the Results section in the online supplement).
We also examined the moderating effect of gender and found that most of the main imaging results were not moderated by gender. However, in the inferior parietal lobule, pre- and post-central gyri, and insula, irritability was related to increased activation in younger boys and decreased activation in older boys (see also the Results section in the online supplement).

Additional Region-of-Interest and Functional Connectivity Analyses

Region-of-interest analyses in the amygdala and striatum revealed positive associations between ARI and striatal activity during the N+1 trial but not during feedback (age moderated some associations) (see the Results section in the online supplement). We also analyzed functional connectivity with the inferior frontal gyrus and amygdala seeds (see the Results section, Tables S5 and S6, and Figures S5–S8 in the online supplement). Notably, we found that higher irritability was related to decreased functional connectivity between the left inferior frontal gyrus and periaqueductal gray (extending to the culmen) during the N+1 trial (see Figure S6 in the online supplement).

Discussion

This study investigated the neural correlates of attention orienting following frustration in a transdiagnostic sample of 195 youths with varying levels of irritability. We used a novel paradigm to model the frustration that irritable youths are prone to experience, particularly when reward is withheld and they are frustrated and asked to adjust their behaviors (e.g., to stop playing video games and start homework instead). We found that irritability was associated with neural activation during attention orienting following a frustrating event but not during processing of the frustrating feedback itself. Two specific findings emerged. First, higher irritability was related to increased activation in multiple frontal-striatal regions independently of age. Second, in other regions, associations between irritability and activation were moderated by age, such that associations were stronger in younger children compared with older children. These findings suggest that promising treatments for irritability may target frontal-striatal regions and that interventions could be prioritized for younger youths with high irritability.
Irritability was associated with dysfunction in the prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex (2022, 36, 37), and striatum (19, 36). This is consistent with previous research in youths and adults with irritability or related phenotypes (anger and trait aggression) (1922, 36, 37). Specifically, while engaged in the attentional part of the task after being frustrated, highly irritable youths exhibited increased activation in multiple frontal regions (anterior cingulate cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and inferior frontal gyrus) implicated in cognitive control, executive attention, and attention orienting (38, 39). Highly irritable youths also showed increased activation in the striatum, which has a regulatory influence on the cortex and is involved in motor control and eye movements as well as in set shifting (40).
Notably, irritability was not associated with performance deficits (e.g., poor accuracy and slower reaction time). Thus, the observed increased activation may reflect a compensatory mechanism (i.e., compared with healthy youths, irritable youths may require more robust recruitment of these regions following frustration to regulate their negative affect, focus on the task at hand, and meet task demands). Importantly, our finding is best explained by irritability and not better attributed to co-occurring anxiety, ADHD, or depressive symptoms. Furthermore, we found no associations between irritability and activation during non-frustration runs. This observation suggests that trait-specific neural correlates manifest in irritable youths when they are frustrated. Such context specificity is consistent with our event-related analysis contrasting activity during performance of the attention-orienting task following frustrating feedback compared with non-frustrating feedback. Because of the insufficient number of incorrect trials, we were unable to examine the neural responses to errors (in which reward is omitted and not expected). An interesting and important question is whether irritability has similar associations with responses to errors, responses to “pure” frustrative nonreward (i.e., in which reward is expected and omitted because of changed contingencies), and responses to frustrative nonreward due to deception (rigged feedback).
Despite comparable performance on a simple attention task, irritable youths exhibited heightened frontal activation following frustration, suggesting a requirement for greater prefrontal cortex engagement to adjust to frustration and achieve performance comparable to that of less irritable youths. Many everyday tasks (e.g., schoolwork, homework, or transitions between activities) are much more cognitively demanding than laboratory tasks, such as the one used here. Therefore, inefficiency in systems that facilitate post-frustration adjustment could lead irritable youths to struggle in daily life. Moreover, whereas many irritable youths in our sample were medicated, the impact of medications on their brain function was not evident in our analysis. This suggests that current medications may fail to normalize the particular neural dysfunction reported here. There is clearly a need for new treatments, including nonpharmacological approaches (e.g., real-time fMRI neurofeedback or transcranial magnetic stimulation targeting frontal-striatal regions). Additionally, given that neural dysfunction associated with irritability was found during attentional processes following frustration, intervention efforts might emphasize strategies that help irritable children allocate attention effectively.
Age moderated the association between irritability and brain activation during attention orienting following frustration in several frontal and posterior regions (anterior cingulate cortex, medial frontal gyrus, cuneus, and precuneus). Specifically, higher irritability was related to increased activation in children and younger adolescents but not older adolescents; the association was particularly strong in young children. This finding is inconsistent with the only previous study that examined the interacting effect of age and irritability, which found that as age increased, higher irritability was associated with more frontal-striatal-thalamic activation (17). This discrepancy could be explained by differences in sample size (N=30 compared with N=195), sample characteristics such as age (4–12 years compared with 8–18 years), and the nature of the sample (nonclinical compared with clinical) as well as fMRI paradigm (emotionally or neutrally valenced video clips compared with a frustrating cued-attention task). Young and highly irritable children may be most susceptible to affectively charged stimuli and may have the greatest difficulty disengaging attention from negatively arousing stimuli. Although these youths did not show performance deficits, their ability to perform the task despite immature neural circuitry may have required prolonged and inefficient computational processes that generated increased regional neural activity (41). Indeed, the increased anterior cingulate cortex activation seen in young children, compared with older youths and adults, may reflect neural inefficiency in cognitive control (41).
Contrary to some previous studies, we did not find an association between irritability and amygdala activation in response to a frustrating event (19, 37). This could be due to differences in frustration paradigms (i.e., a block design [37] compared with an event-related design, or no jitter [19] compared with jitter between attentional and feedback portions of the task [the latter allows for separation of attention orienting and feedback processing]). Alternatively, the unreliability of amygdala activation may hamper replication (42). It is noteworthy that our null amygdala finding is consistent with a recent study that also adopted a dimensional approach to examine irritability and brain function during frustration (20).
There are several limitations to our study. First, our findings may apply only to the disorders sampled (disruptive mood dysregulation disorder, anxiety disorder, and ADHD) and not to other disorders (e.g., depression and bipolar disorder) in which irritability is also common. Second, given the high correlation between chronological age and puberty status, we used age as a proxy for development. Future work is needed to directly examine the effect of puberty. Third, although our cross-sectional design is a helpful starting point to understand brain function over development, only longitudinal studies can elucidate individual differences in developmental trajectories. Fourth, as in most studies of youths with severe impairments, medication may confound the results; however, our post hoc analyses do not support confounding by medication. Finally, this study used raw, reported scores of irritability symptoms. Other phenotyping approaches (e.g., latent variable modeling) may provide additional perspectives.

Conclusions

In a sample characterized by a mix of mood, anxiety, and ADHD symptoms in the clinically impairing range, we found unique associations of irritability with neural systems mediating attention orienting, top-down regulation of emotions, and motor execution following frustrative nonreward. These associations were not attributed to co-occurring ADHD, anxiety, or depressive symptoms. Although most associations were independent of age, dysfunction in the anterior cingulate cortex and some posterior regions was more pronounced in young children with irritability. The neural dysfunction did not seem to be altered by medications, highlighting a need for new treatments, including nonpharmacological approaches, that target the common and impairing symptom of irritability. Because associations between irritability and brain function were found during attentional processes following frustration, intervention efforts could target strategies that help children with irritability regulate the negative affect and arousal elicited by frustrating events and flexibly shift their attention to focus on the task at hand.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the children and families who participated in this study as well as the staff of the Emotion and Development Branch and the Scientific and Statistical Computing Core at NIMH. The authors also thank Anderson Winkler, M.D., D.Phil. (from the NIMH Emotion and Development Branch) for assistance on permutation analysis of linear models and threshold-free cluster enhancement analysis, Dan Barlow for programming support, and Gang Chen, Ph.D., for imaging statistics guidance.

Footnote

The computational resources of the NIH High Performing Computation system’s Biowulf cluster was used in this study (http://hpc.nih.gov).

Supplementary Material

File (appi.ajp.2018.18040491.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Brotman MA, Kircanski K, Stringaris A, et al: Irritability in youth: a translational model. Am J Psychiatry 2017; 174:520–532
2.
Leibenluft E: Pediatric irritability: a systems neuroscience approach. Trends Cogn Sci 2017; 21:277–289
3.
Vidal-Ribas P, Brotman MA, Valdivieso I, et al: The status of irritability in psychiatry: a conceptual and quantitative review. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2016; 55:556–570
4.
Roy AK, Lopes V, Klein RG: Disruptive mood dysregulation disorder: a new diagnostic approach to chronic irritability in youth. Am J Psychiatry 2014; 171:918–924
5.
Stringaris A, Vidal-Ribas P, Brotman MA, et al: Practitioner review: definition, recognition, and treatment challenges of irritability in young people. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2018; 59:721–739
6.
Amsel A: The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. Psychol Bull 1958; 55:102–119
7.
Wakschlag LS, Estabrook R, Petitclerc A, et al: Clinical implications of a dimensional approach: the normal:abnormal spectrum of early irritability. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2015; 54:626–634
8.
Pagliaccio D, Pine DS, Barch DM, et al: Irritability trajectories, cortical thickness, and clinical outcomes in a sample enriched for preschool depression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2018; 57:336–342.e6
9.
John OP, Gross JJ: Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. J Pers 2004; 72:1301–1333
10.
Posner MI, Rothbart MK, Sheese BE, et al: Developing attention: behavioral and brain mechanisms. Adv Neurosci (Hindawi) 2014; vol 2014, article ID 405094 (https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/405094)
11.
Cohen AO, Breiner K, Steinberg L, et al: When is an adolescent an adult? assessing cognitive control in emotional and nonemotional contexts. Psychol Sci 2016; 27:549–562
12.
Ahmed SP, Bittencourt-Hewitt A, Sebastian CL: Neurocognitive bases of emotion regulation development in adolescence. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2015; 15:11–25
13.
Ordaz SJ, Foran W, Velanova K, et al: Longitudinal growth curves of brain function underlying inhibitory control through adolescence. J Neurosci 2013; 33:18109–18124
14.
Petersen SE, Posner MI: The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu Rev Neurosci 2012; 35:73–89
15.
Luby JL, Agrawal A, Belden A, et al: Developmental trajectories of the orbitofrontal cortex and anhedonia in middle childhood and risk for substance use in adolescence in a longitudinal sample of depressed and healthy preschoolers. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175:1010–1021
16.
Tamnes CK, Walhovd KB, Torstveit M, et al: Performance monitoring in children and adolescents: a review of developmental changes in the error-related negativity and brain maturation. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2013; 6:1–13
17.
Karim HT, Perlman SB: Neurodevelopmental maturation as a function of irritable temperament: insights from a naturalistic emotional video viewing paradigm. Hum Brain Mapp 2017; 38:5307–5321
18.
Wakschlag LS, Perlman SB, Blair RJ, et al: The neurodevelopmental basis of early childhood disruptive behavior: irritable and callous phenotypes as exemplars. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175:114–130
19.
Deveney CM, Connolly ME, Haring CT, et al: Neural mechanisms of frustration in chronically irritable children. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170:1186–1194
20.
Grabell AS, Li Y, Barker JW, et al: Evidence of non-linear associations between frustration-related prefrontal cortex activation and the normal:abnormal spectrum of irritability in young children. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2018; 46:137–147
21.
Perlman SB, Jones BM, Wakschlag LS, et al: Neural substrates of child irritability in typically developing and psychiatric populations. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2015; 14:71–80
22.
Rich BA, Carver FW, Holroyd T, et al: Different neural pathways to negative affect in youth with pediatric bipolar disorder and severe mood dysregulation. J Psychiatr Res 2011; 45:1283–1294
23.
Poldrack RA, Baker CI, Durnez J, et al: Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat Rev Neurosci 2017; 18:115–126
24.
Posner MI, Walker JA, Friedrich FJ, et al: Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. J Neurosci 1984; 4:1863–1874
25.
Kircanski K, White LK, Tseng WL, et al: A latent variable approach to differentiating neural mechanisms of irritability and anxiety in youth. JAMA Psychiatry 2018; 75:631–639
26.
Kircanski K, Zhang S, Stringaris A, et al: Empirically derived patterns of psychiatric symptoms in youth: a latent profile analysis. J Affect Disord 2017; 216:109–116
27.
Shaffer D, Gould MS, Brasic J, et al: A Children’s Global Assessment Scale (CGAS). Arch Gen Psychiatry 1983; 40:1228–1231
28.
Stringaris A, Goodman R, Ferdinando S, et al: The Affective Reactivity Index: a concise irritability scale for clinical and research settings. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2012; 53:1109–1117
29.
Birmaher B, Brent DA, Chiappetta L, et al: Psychometric properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): a replication study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38:1230–1236
30.
Conners CK, Sitarenios G, Parker JDA, et al: The revised Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1998; 26:257–268
31.
Tseng W-L, Moroney E, Machlin L, et al: Test-retest reliability and validity of a frustration paradigm and irritability measures. J Affect Disord 2017; 212:38–45
32.
Chen G, Adleman NE, Saad ZS, et al: Applications of multivariate modeling to neuroimaging group analysis: a comprehensive alternative to univariate general linear model. Neuroimage 2014; 99:571–588
33.
Cox RW, Chen G, Glen DR, et al: fMRI clustering in AFNI: false-positive rates redux. Brain Connect 2017; 7:152–171
34.
Smith SM, Nichols TE: Threshold-free cluster enhancement: addressing problems of smoothing, threshold dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 2009; 44:83–98
35.
Kovacs M: Children’s Depression Inventory. North Tonawanda, NY, Multi-Health Systems, 1992
36.
Abler B, Walter H, Erk S: Neural correlates of frustration. Neuroreport 2005; 16:669–672
37.
Pawliczek CM, Derntl B, Kellermann T, et al: Anger under control: neural correlates of frustration as a function of trait aggression. PLoS One 2013; 8:e78503
38.
MacDonald AW 3rd, Cohen JD, Stenger VA, et al: Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science 2000; 288:1835–1838
39.
Corbetta M, Shulman GL: Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002; 3:201–215
40.
Ravizza SM, Ciranni MA: Contributions of the prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia to set shifting. J Cogn Neurosci 2002; 14:472–483
41.
Luna B, Padmanabhan A, O’Hearn K: What has fMRI told us about the development of cognitive control through adolescence? Brain Cogn 2010; 72:101–113
42.
van den Bulk BG, Koolschijn PCMP, Meens PHF, et al: How stable is activation in the amygdala and prefrontal cortex in adolescence? a study of emotional face processing across three measurements. Dev Cogn Neurosci 2013; 4:65–76
43.
Vul E, Harris C, Winkielman P, et al: Puzzlingly high correlations in fMRI studies of emotion, personality, and social cognition. Perspect Psychol Sci 2009; 4:274–290

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
Go to American Journal of Psychiatry
American Journal of Psychiatry
Pages: 67 - 76
PubMed: 30336704

History

Received: 30 April 2018
Revision received: 3 July 2018
Accepted: 30 July 2018
Published online: 19 October 2018
Published in print: January 01, 2019

Keywords

  1. Irritability
  2. fMRI
  3. Age
  4. Frustrative Non-Reward
  5. Attention Orienting

Authors

Details

Wan-Ling Tseng, Ph.D. [email protected]
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Christen M. Deveney, Ph.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Joel Stoddard, M.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Katharina Kircanski, Ph.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Anna E. Frackman, M.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Jennifer Y. Yi, M.A.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Derek Hsu, M.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Elizabeth Moroney, B.A.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Laura Machlin, B.A.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Laura Donahue, B.A.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Alexandra Roule, B.A.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Gretchen Perhamus, B.A.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Richard C. Reynolds, M.S.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Roxann Roberson-Nay, Ph.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
John M. Hettema, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Kenneth E. Towbin, M.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Argyris Stringaris, M.D., Ph.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Daniel S. Pine, M.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Melissa A. Brotman, Ph.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.
Ellen Leibenluft, M.D.
From the Department of Health and Human Services, Emotion and Development Branch and Scientific and Statistical Computing Core, NIMH, Bethesda, Md.; the Department of Psychiatry and the Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University, Richmond; the Department of Psychology, Wellesley College, Wellesley, Mass.; the University of Colorado School of Medicine, Aurora, Colo.; the Department of Medicine, Stanford University, Stanford, Calif.; the Departments of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill; Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta; the Department of Psychology, University of California at Los Angeles; and University of Michigan Medical School, Ann Arbor.

Notes

Send correspondence to Dr. Tseng ([email protected]).

Competing Interests

Drs. Tseng and Stoddard have received research grant support from NIMH (grant K99MH110570 to Dr. Tseng and grant K23MH113731 to Dr. Stoddard). The other authors report no financial relationships with commercial interests.

Funding Information

National Institute of Mental Health10.13039/100000025: ZIA-MH-002781, ZIA-MH002778, ZIA44 MH002786
Supported by the Intramural Research Program of NIMH, conducted under projects ZIA-MH002778 (clinical protocol NCT00006177), ZIA-MH002781 (clinical protocol NCT00018057), and ZIA-MH002786 (clinical protocol NCT00025935).

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

PPV Articles - American Journal of Psychiatry

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share