Skip to main content

Abstract

Although it has long been recognized that many individuals with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) also have difficulties with emotion regulation, no consensus has been reached on how to conceptualize this clinically challenging domain. The authors examine the current literature using both quantitative and qualitative methods. Three key findings emerge. First, emotion dysregulation is prevalent in ADHD throughout the lifespan and is a major contributor to impairment. Second, emotion dysregulation in ADHD may arise from deficits in orienting toward, recognizing, and/or allocating attention to emotional stimuli; these deficits implicate dysfunction within a striato-amygdalo-medial prefrontal cortical network. Third, while current treatments for ADHD often also ameliorate emotion dysregulation, a focus on this combination of symptoms reframes clinical questions and could stimulate novel therapeutic approaches. The authors then consider three models to explain the overlap between emotion dysregulation and ADHD: emotion dysregulation and ADHD are correlated but distinct dimensions; emotion dysregulation is a core diagnostic feature of ADHD; and the combination constitutes a nosological entity distinct from both ADHD and emotion dysregulation alone. The differing predictions from each model can guide research on the much-neglected population of patients with ADHD and emotion dysregulation.
(Reprinted from the American Journal of Psychiatry 2014; 171:276–293, with permission from American Psychiatric Association Publishing)
It has long been recognized that emotion dysregulation is common in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders, including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Indeed, in the early conceptualization of ADHD as reflecting “minimal brain damage,” emotion dysregulation was placed along with inattention among the cardinal symptoms (1). Only with the publication of DSM-III did emotional symptoms became an “associated feature” rather than a diagnostic criterion of ADHD. Renewed interest in this area makes timely a review of the overlap of emotion dysregulation with ADHD, focusing on prevalence, pathophysiology, and treatment.
In line with previous theorists, we define emotion regulation as an individual’s ability to modify an emotional state so as to promote adaptive, goal-oriented behaviors (2). It encompasses the processes that allow the individual to select, attend to, and appraise emotionally arousing stimuli, and to do so flexibly. These processes trigger behavioral and physiological responses that can be modulated in line with goals. Emotion dysregulation arises when these adaptive processes are impaired, leading to behavior that defeats the individual’s interests. It encompasses 1) emotional expressions and experiences that are excessive in relation to social norms and are context inappropriate; 2) rapid, poorly controlled shifts in emotion (lability); and 3) the anomalous allocation of attention to emotional stimuli. Here, we focus on the clinical expression of emotion dysregulation as irritability, which is often linked with reactive aggression and temper outbursts (35).
Emotion dysregulation is a dimensional trait that is not unique to ADHD; rather, it undercuts the traditional divide between internalizing and externalizing diagnoses and, indeed, may partly explain their high correlation (6). For example, a study that contrasted 105 irritable, emotionally dysregulated children with ADHD and 395 nonirritable children with ADHD found higher rates not only of oppositional defiant disorder but also of depression and dysthymia in the group with irritability (7).
Emotion dysregulation is also not synonymous with any single DSM-5 disorder. For example, of the three symptom clusters in oppositional defiant disorder—angry/irritable mood, defiant behavior, and vindictiveness—only the first plausibly reflects dysregulated emotions (8). In its extreme form, emotion dysregulation is likely to emerge as a major etiological factor behind the frequent, severe temper outbursts and irritability of the new DSM-5 diagnosis of disruptive mood dysregulation disorder. However, emotion dysregulation is a dimensional entity, not a categorical diagnosis, and here we consider the full spectrum of emotion dysregulation within ADHD, not just extremes. Thus, we include individuals with emotion dysregulation who do not meet criteria for any DSM diagnoses beyond ADHD.
We focus on emotion dysregulation itself, rather than on diagnoses that may include emotion dysregulation and be comorbid with ADHD, because it is a simpler symptom construct that is familiar to clinicians and, consistent with the Research Domain Criteria initiative, may be more readily tied to underlying neurobiological mechanisms.

Method

We conducted a literature search for relevant articles published before January 1, 2013 (details of the search are available in the data supplement that accompanies the online edition of this article). We summarized data quantitatively where possible. Meta-analyses were possible for studies of aggressive behavior (920), emotion recognition (12, 2136), and delay aversion/reward valuation (35, 3750). As all the outcomes included in the meta-analysis were continuous, we calculated standardized mean differences. We used a random-effects model to generate a pooled effect size and confidence intervals with the inverse variance method. The remaining studies are reviewed qualitatively under the headings of prevalence, pathophysiology, and treatment.

Results

Prevalence

Childhood.

Most epidemiological research has focused on children and has found a strong association between ADHD and emotion dysregulation (35, 5156) (Table 1). A population study of 5,326 youths (51) found mood lability in 38% of children with ADHD, ten times the population rate. Elevated rates were observed in children without comorbid ADHD, and similar rates were seen in children with noncomorbid oppositional defiant disorder. Research on the Child Behavior Checklist “dysregulation profile” based on parent-reported problems with mood and aggression in youths who also have attention problems shows community rates of 1%–5%, compatible with high rates of emotion dysregulation among those likely to have ADHD (62). Clinic-based studies in youths with ADHD similarly report prevalence estimates of emotion dysregulation between 24% and 50%.
TABLE 1. Prevalence Estimates of Emotion Dysregulation in Children and Adults With ADHDa
StudyParticipantsDefinition of “Emotion Dysregulation”Impairment CriterionFindings
Children and adolescents
Stringaris and Goodman (51)Population based; N=5326Parent and self-report of emotional labilitySeverity ratings of symptoms occurring “a lot”Parent rating of impairing emotional lability: ADHD alone, 38% (RRR=12 compared with unaffected); ODD alone, 42% (RRR=14.7); self-report: ADHD alone, 27% (RRR=6.9); ODD alone, 14% (RRR=3.0)
Sobanski et al. (52)Family based; ADHD, N=216; siblings, N=142Conners emotional lability index, parent and teacher ratings of unpredictable mood changes, temper tantrums; tearfulness; low frustration tolerance3 SD above population norms25% of ADHD probands had emotional lability >3 SD above population norms
Anastopoulos et al. (53)Family based; ADHD, N=216; siblings, N=142Conners emotional lability index (see above)Above 65th percentile of population normsElevated levels: ADHD, 47%; unaffected, 15%
Spencer et al. (54)Clinic based; ADHD, N=197; controls, N=224Parent report of “dysregulation profile” based on Child Behavior Checklist subscales of attention problems, anxiety/depression, and aggressionScores 1–2 SD above norms (above 2 SD, considered bipolar phenotype and excluded)ADHD, 44% with dysregulation profile; controls, 2%
Sjöwall et al. (35)Clinic based; ADHD, N=102; controls, N=102Parent report of child’s ability to regulate specific emotionsNot givenADHD showed significant impairment compared with controls in regulating all emotions
Strine et al. (55)Population based; history of ADHD, N=512; no history of ADHD, N=8,169Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire, parent report of emotional and conduct problems, including: often loses temper, often unhappy (also clingy, fearful, somatic complaints, and having worries)Parent rating of each symptom’s impactEmotional problems: history of ADHD, 23%; no history of ADHD, 6.3%
Becker et al. (56)Clinic based; ADHD, N=1,450Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire, parent report of emotional problems (see above)Based on U.K. population norms40% of boys and 49% of girls had abnormally high levels of emotional problems
Adult studies    
Able et al. (57)Population based (N=21,000); diagnosed ADHD, N=198; likely ADHD (based on self-report scale), N=752; controls, N=199Self-report of tendency to become angry, disagree, or be critical of others; self-report of degree to which others evoke feelings of angerNot givenBoth diagnosed and likely ADHD subjects more likely to express anger, to engage in conflict, and to have been the target of anger or intimidating behavior
Barkley and Fischer (58)Clinic based; ADHD, N=55; controls, N=75Self-report of items reflecting emotional impulsivity (taken from the Behavior Rating of Executive Functioning)Symptom occurs “often”Impatient: ADHD, 72%; controls, 3%; quick to anger: ADHD, 65%; controls, 6%; easily frustrated: ADHD, 85%; controls, 7%; emotionally overexcitable: ADHD, 70%; controls, 6%; easily excitable: ADHD, 73%; controls, 14%
Reimherr et al. (59)Clinic based; ADHD, N=536 (enrolled in treatment trials)Self-report of items from the Wender-Reimherr Adult Attention Disorder Rating Scale: irritability and outbursts; short, unpredictable mood shifts; emotional overreactivity2 SD above population norms32% met criteria for emotion dysregulation
Reimherr et al. (60)Clinic based; ADHD, N=47 (enrolled in treatment trial)Wender-Reimherr Adult Attention Disorder Rating Scale (see above)2 SD above population norms78% met criteria for emotion dysregulation
Surman et al. (61)Clinic based; ADHD, N=206; controls, N=123Barkley’s self-report scale: quick to anger, loses temper, argumentative, angry; easily frustrated, touchy; overreactive emotionally, easily excitedAbove 95th percentile on population normsMet criteria for emotion dysregulation: ADHD, 55%; controls, 3%
a
ADHD=attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ODD=oppositional defiant disorder; RRR=relative risk ratio.
Reactive aggression may reflect emotion dysregulation (5). Our meta-analysis found consistent elevation in measures of aggressive behavior in ADHD compared with non-ADHD populations, associated with a large effect size (1.92, 95% CI=0.95–2.89) (Figure 1A). In the general population, the correlation is higher between aggression and hyperactivity-impulsivity (0.60–0.83) than between aggression and inattention (0.20–0.56) (6). In clinical populations, emotion dysregulation is commonly associated with either symptom domain (52, 56).
FIGURE 1. Forest Plots With Standardized Mean Difference Between ADHD and Comparison Groups in Measures of Aggression, Emotion Recognition, and Reward Processinga
a In panel A, more aggressive behavior is seen in the ADHD groups (the effect size for boys in the Abikoff study [9] was 14). In panel B, emotion recognition deficits are seen in ADHD. In panel C, reward processing is measured by the tendency to prefer immediate small rewards over larger delayed ones; the ADHD participants show a tendency to prefer immediate, small rewards. Further details are provided in the online data supplement.
b SMD=standardized mean difference, inverse variance, random effects, with 95% confidence intervals.
Notably, behaviors reflecting emotion dysregulation can be reliably provoked among those with ADHD using paradigms that induce frustration (see Table S1 in the online data supplement). Children with ADHD show more negative affect and temper outbursts than do comparison subjects during challenging tasks. This consistency among studies is also notable, as different paradigms and behavioral measures were employed.

Infancy and early childhood.

Modest correlations (0.10–0.37) are reported between difficult infantile temperamental characteristics, such as being fussy, angry, or difficult to control, and ADHD arising later in childhood (6367) (Figure 2; see also Table S2 in the online data supplement). A longitudinal study of 7,140 children found that while temperamental emotionality at age 3 predicted comorbid ADHD and internalizing disorders at age 7, activity level predicted comorbid ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder (68). A second longitudinal study found that infants who developed hyperactive symptoms alone did not differ temperamentally from typical infants, whereas those who ultimately developed both ADHD and aggressive symptoms were uncooperative and irritable from infancy onward (69). In short, a difficult early temperament with prominent negative emotionality is modestly linked with later ADHD combined with emotion dysregulation.
FIGURE 2. Correlations Between Infantile Temperament and Later Externalizing and ADHD Symptomsa
a n.s.=not significant.
*p<0.05. **p<0.01.

Longitudinal studies.

Most studies following children with ADHD into adulthood have focused on DSM-IV diagnoses, without considering emotion dysregulation per se, and have found elevated rates of adult disruptive and antisocial disorders and, less consistently, mood and anxiety disorders (70). One study defined emotion dysregulation as a moderate elevation (one to two standard deviations above the mean) on the combined Child Behavior Checklist subscales for attention problems, aggressive behavior, and anxious/depressed (71). In that study, such emotion dysregulation in 79 children with ADHD was associated 4 years later with more psychiatric comorbidities, greater social impairment, and ADHD persistence, compared with 98 children with ADHD without emotion dysregulation and 204 children without ADHD. A population-based study of 2,076 children (72) found that those matching the Child Behavior Checklist dysregulation profile had higher rates of anxiety disorders and disruptive behavior disorders in adulthood compared with those who did not match the dysregulation profile (72).

Adult studies.

Earlier concepts of adult ADHD included emotion dysregulation as a defining feature (73). This model has been supported to some extent by recent clinic-based studies reporting impairing emotion dysregulation in 34%–70% of adults with ADHD (5761), although population-based studies are needed (Table 1). Aggressive behaviors are also prominent. In a population study contrasting 950 adults with diagnosed or likely ADHD and 20,000 unaffected adults (57), the ADHD group had higher self-ratings of interpersonal conflict and negative, conflictual social ties. Other cross-sectional studies have compared adults whose childhood ADHD has remitted and those whose ADHD has persisted. In one such comparison (58), 55 adults with persistent ADHD showed higher rates of emotion dysregulation (42%–72%, depending on specific symptoms) than 80 adults with remitted ADHD (23%–45%), although both groups differed from healthy subjects. This suggests a degree of developmental coherence: as symptoms of ADHD improve, so may emotion dysregulation.

Impairment.

The combination of ADHD and emotion dysregulation represents a major source of impairment. In a study of 1,500 children (74), emotional problems were found to have a greater impact than hyperactivity and inattention on well-being and self-esteem. Individuals with ADHD and emotion dysregulation were significantly more impaired in peer relationships, family life, occupational attainment, and academic performance than those with ADHD alone (75), and this result held after controlling for comorbid disorders, including oppositional defiant disorder (76).
In summary, emotion dysregulation is found in some 25%–45% of children and 30%–70% of adults with ADHD. It represents a major source of impairment and presages a poor clinical outcome.

Pathophysiology

Several psychological and neural processes may underpin the overlap between ADHD and emotion dysregulation. Recent models describe both “bottom-up” processes that support or influence emotion regulation and “top-down” processes, such as the allocation of attention to emotionally arousing stimuli (77, 78). Most studies reviewed in this section have either excluded individuals with comorbid diagnoses (including oppositional defiant disorder) or controlled for comorbidities, ensuring that the anomalies pertain to ADHD rather than to other disorders (Table 2).
TABLE 2. Summary of Functional MRI Studies of Emotion Perception, Reward Processing, and the Allocation of Attention to Emotional Stimulia
StudyParticipantsTaskBehavioral resultsfMRI results
Emotion perception and recognition
Brotman et al. (79)ADHD with no comorbidity, N=18; severe mood dysregulation, N=29 (24 with ADHD); bipolar affective disorder, N=43 (20 with ADHD); healthy, N=37Rating of fear, nose width, and passive viewing of neutral, fearful, happy, and angry facesRating of fear in neutral faces: severe mood dysregulation = bipolar > healthy; ADHD did not differ from any groupLeft amygdala activity during fear ratings: ADHD > healthy = bipolar > severe mood dysregulation
Marsh et al. (80)ADHD with no comorbidity, N=12; callous-unemotional traits, N=12; healthy, N=12Gender judgments on fearful, neutral, and angry facesNo group differences in accuracy; ADHD had slower reaction timesAmygdala activity in ADHD during fear processing did not differ from healthy
Posner et al. (81)ADHD, N=15 (mix of medication naive and receiving psychostimulants; some had ODD, although the number is unclear); healthy, N=15Subliminal presentation of fearful face followed by supraliminal presentation of neutral expression on the same face; postscan face memory testNo group differencesGreater activity in medication-naive ADHD in amygdala and stronger functional connectivity with lateral prefrontal cortex (BA47)
Herpertz et al. (24)ADHD without comorbidity, N=13; conduct disorder, N=22 (16 with ADHD); healthy, N=22Passive viewing of negative, positive, and neutral scenesSubjects with conduct disorder rated emotional pictures as less arousing than did other groupsIncreased left amygdala activation in conduct disorder with ADHD, not ADHD alone; ADHD alone had decreased insula activation to negative faces
Schlochtermeier et al. (82)Adults treated in childhood for ADHD with no comorbidity, N=10; adults with childhood ADHD, medication naive, N=10; healthy, N=10Rating of positive and negative picturesAdults with ADHD treated in childhood rated neutral pictures as more pleasant than medication naive and healthy subjectsDecreased ventral striatum and subgenual cingulate activation in medication-naive adults with history of ADHD; ADHD treated in childhood did not differ from healthy
Malisza et al. (29)ADHD, N=9; autism, N=9; healthy, N=9View happy and angry faces and respond to happyAccuracy: autism < ADHD = healthyADHD had less fusiform, temporal poles activity than healthy; ADHD showed same amygdala activity as healthy; autism showed less amygdala activity than other two groups
Reward processing
Ströhle et al. (83)Adult ADHD without comorbidity, N=10; controls, N=10Monetary incentive delayNo group differencesDecreased ventral striatum activation in ADHD during reward anticipation, and increased orbitofrontal activation during reward receipt
Plichta et al. (38)Adult ADHD without comorbidity, N=14; controls, N=12Delayed discounting task (choose between immediate small and delayed large rewards)No group differencesDecreased ventral striatum activation in ADHD during processing of both immediate and delayed rewards; within subjects, delayed reward in ADHD associated with increased activity of amygdala and caudate
Scheres et al. (84)Adolescent ADHD, N=11; controls, N=11Monetary incentive delayNo group differencesDecreased ventral striatal activity in ADHD during reward anticipation
Stoy et al. (85)Adult ADHD, N=24 (analyzed as remitted versus persistent, and as history of childhood treatment with psychostimulants versus medication naive); controls, N=12Monetary incentive delayNo group differencesDecreased insula activation during outcome of loss avoidance in medication-naive adults compared with other groups
Rubia et al. (86)Childhood ADHD on and off psychostimulants, N=13 (1 with comorbid ODD); healthy, N=13Rewarded continuous performance taskNo difference between medicated ADHD and healthy; trend to worse performance in unmedicated ADHDUnmedicated ADHD showed orbitofrontal hyperactivation during reward receipt, normalized by psychostimulants
Control of attention to emotional stimuli
Passarotti et al. (87)Adolescent ADHD without comorbidity (N=14); bipolar disorder, N=23; healthy, N=19Working memory task using angry, happy, and neutral facesAccuracy: healthy > ADHD > bipolarADHD compared with healthy: decreased prefrontal and striatal activation to angry faces, increased to happy; ADHD compared with bipolar: similar cortical anomalies, more prominent subcortical anomalies in bipolar
Passarotti et al. (88)Adolescent ADHD without comorbidity (N=15); bipolar disorder, N=17; healthy, N=15Emotional Stroop testBipolar and ADHD slower than healthy; more interference from positive distractors in bipolar and from negative distractors in ADHDFor negative versus neutral words: gradient of ventrolateral prefrontal cortical activation: ADHD < healthy < bipolar; both ADHD and bipolar showed more dorsolateral prefrontal and parietal activation than healthy
Posner et al. (89)Adolescent ADHD, on and off psychostimulants, N=15; healthy, N=15Emotional Stroop test Medication-free ADHD showed medial prefrontal hyperactivity with positive and hypoactivity with negative distractors; normalized on psychostimulants
a
ADHD=attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; ODD=oppositional defiant disorder.

Bottom-up psychological mechanisms.

Two basic processes affect emotion regulation: orienting to emotionally salient stimuli, and the evaluation of signals for reward. In order for emotion to be regulated, posterior attention systems must both detect salient stimuli and signal that control is needed (77, 90). Evidence suggests anomalies in early orienting to emotional stimuli in ADHD. In healthy individuals, affectively charged stimuli receive enhanced early sensory encoding, detectable by electrophysiological markers. Two studies (91, 92) found that this effect is reduced in adults with ADHD when viewing positive, but not negative stimuli; this would be expected to cause overperception of negative stimuli. The studies further linked these early processing deficits with self-rated emotional lability. Additionally, whereas the startle reflex is typically accentuated by precursive positive stimuli and attenuated by negative stimuli, this effect is lost in adults with ADHD, which constitutes further evidence of abnormal early processing of emotional stimuli in ADHD (93). Likewise, the rapid and accurate recognition of emotions in human faces or voices is central to well-regulated behavior; emotional misperception is linked with aberrant emotional responses, and misperception can itself result from emotion dysregulation (94, 95). Studies of emotion labeling have found moderate impairments in ADHD, with our meta-analysis producing an effect size of 0.65 (95% CI=0.48–0.81) (Figure 1B).
The evaluation of emotionally salient stimuli has also been studied in relation to the evaluation of signals for potential reward. A preference for immediate small rewards over larger delayed ones, even when such choice defeats one’s own goals and desires, is held to be a hallmark of impulsivity, reflecting an aversion to delayed reward (96, 97). Our meta-analysis found that ADHD was also moderately associated with this preference (effect size, 0.6, 95% CI=0.40–0.79), albeit with considerable heterogeneity in results (35, 37) (Figure 1C). This style of reward processing can be construed as a contributor to emotion dysregulation, as it may reflect anomalous activity in limbic regions that are pivotal in emotion processing. Equally, the preference for immediate small rewards might also reflect failures in top-down regulatory mechanisms, such as the ability to hold longer-term goals in mind or to exert cognitive control to suppress the arousing value of immediate incentives (78, 98, 99). Thus, anomalies in reward evaluation provide further, albeit indirect, evidence for dysregulation of emotion systems in ADHD.

Top-down regulatory processes.

Parasympathetic response is considered one gauge of regulatory functioning (100). In typically developing children, autonomic nervous system function tracks the valence of emotional stimuli and task demands, with greater top-down regulatory activity when stimuli are negative rather than positive (100). In children with ADHD, this ability to adjust top-down regulation in response to different emotional stimuli was partially lost, based on physiological indicators of regulation.
Another way to assess the recruitment of regulatory resources is to consider the allocation of attention itself to emotional stimuli. Just as emotion regulation requires the ability to recruit autonomic responses, it also relies on the ability to direct attention toward or away from emotional stimuli so as to maintain emotional homeostasis or maintain focus on a goal (101). This ability can be assessed by incorporating an affective dimension in a cognitive paradigm. For example, in the emotional Stroop task, individuals must deflect attention away from the emotional properties, such as emotional expression, and attend to a nonemotional feature, such as eye color. This manipulation exacerbates the performance deficits already evident in ADHD, suggesting that performance drops off more steeply than it does in typical individuals under emotional challenge (87, 89).
Finally, given that ADHD is associated with poor higher-order cognitive control even in the absence of emotionally salient stimuli, what role does poor cognitive control play in emotion dysregulation in ADHD? Evidence suggests a modest connection but not isomorphism. For example, in 49 boys with and without ADHD, cognitive control, indexed by response inhibition, accounted for 11% of the variance in “dysregulated” behavior during a frustrating task (102). A larger study of 424 children with ADHD and their siblings (37) found that while a range of neuropsychological variables correlated with emotional lability, this link was not direct but was mediated almost entirely by the severity of ADHD symptoms.
In summary, emotion dysregulation in ADHD may arise from deficits at multiple levels. At the most basic level, there are anomalies in orienting to emotional stimuli and reward valuation. This is combined with failings in top-down psychological processes, such as the allocation of attention to emotional stimuli. Meanwhile, deficits in cognitive processes, including working memory and response inhibition, may contribute to emotion dysregulation, but by themselves they do not seem to explain its presence in ADHD.

Neural mechanisms.

It is useful to distinguish between regions mediating bottom-up responses to emotional stimuli—specifically the amygdala, ventral striatum, and orbitofrontal cortex—and top-down cortical regions controlling the allocation of attentional resources in emotionally arousing contexts (77, 103). In ADHD, functional imaging studies have yielded disparate findings, possibly because of differences in tasks and sample characteristics and limited power to detect effects in smaller studies, but nonetheless some themes emerge (Table 2 and Figure 3).
FIGURE 3. Neural Circuits Implicated in Emotion Dysregulation in ADHDa
a The circuitry that underpins deficits in early orienting to emotional stimuli and their perception is shown in red. Regions that interface between emotional and cognitive circuits, allocating attention to emotional stimuli, are shown in yellow. Circuitry implicated in cognitive control, motor planning, and attention is shown in blue. ACC=anterior cingulate cortex; pOFC=posterior orbitofrontal cortex; PFC=prefrontal cortex; VLPFC=ventrolateral prefrontal cortex.
Amygdala activation during emotion processing in ADHD has received some research attention. The larger studies find amygdala hyperactivation in ADHD, during both the subliminal perception of fearful expressions and while subjects rated their fear of neutral faces, although results are mixed (24, 29, 7982) (Table 2; see also the online data supplement). Amygdala hyperactivation has also been reported in ADHD during the processing of delayed rewards, perhaps consistent with the delay aversion found in some behavioral studies (38, 8385). Deficits in early processing of visual emotional stimuli and in the modulation of the startle reflex, described above, also suggest amygdala dysfunction in ADHD. These functional deficits align with reports of amygdala structural abnormalities in ADHD, including surface morphology and dopamine receptor density (104).
The orbitofrontal cortex, which has rich interconnections with the amygdala, the thalamus, and multiple cortical regions, is pivotal in emotion regulation and reward representations (77, 105). Some data suggest orbitofrontal anatomic anomalies (106) and abnormal activation during the anticipation and receipt of rewards in ADHD. There is also decreased connectivity between the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex, reflected in a loss of the typical correlation between the volumes of these structures (104).
The ventral striatum is the third important hub in the bottom-up circuitry, partly by virtue of its role in mediating positive affect and reward processing (107). Functional neuroimaging studies find reduced ventral striatum responsiveness in ADHD during the anticipation (and receipt) of rewards, thus contributing to aversion to delay (Table 2). By examining brain activity at rest in ADHD, two groups have reported both increased functional connectivity between the ventral striatum and the orbitofrontal/ventromedial prefrontal cortex and decreased connectivity between these regions and cortical attentional control regions (108, 109). Thus, evidence suggests dysfunction in a network encompassing the amygdala, ventral striatum, and orbitofrontal cortex that processes emotional stimuli and is implicated in emotion regulation.
With regard to cortical regions, in healthy subjects the addition of an emotional dimension to cognitive tasks usually boosts top-down prefrontal cortical activation (particularly in ventrolateral, medial prefrontal, and anterior cingulate cortical regions) and diminishes subcortical activity (77). These patterns are partly lost in ADHD. Specifically, when negative stimuli are added to a working memory task, performance deficits in ADHD are associated with hypoactivation in prefrontal control regions, including the ventrolateral, orbitofrontal, and medial prefrontal cortices. However, when positive stimuli are used, ADHD patients show hyperactivation in these regions (87). Similarly, two independent studies using the emotional Stroop task (88, 89) found hypoactivation in ADHD in the right medial and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex while processing negative distractors but hyperactivation in the left medial prefrontal cortex while processing positive distractors. Such work represents the first stage in charting the neural basis of dysregulated attentional control in ADHD in the presence of emotional stimuli.
In summary, emotion dysregulation in ADHD implicates dysfunction in the amygdala, ventral striatum, and orbitofrontal cortex, which could be regarded as the bottom-up contributor. Regions at the interface of cognition and emotion (the medial and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex) may underpin the abnormal allocation of attention to emotional stimuli and could thus be regarded as the major top-down contributor to emotion dysregulation in ADHD (Figure 2). Higher cortical centers involved in motor control (supplementary motor areas, motor cortex), monitoring for salient stimuli (temporoparietal junction, frontal operculum), and shifting attention flexibly (frontal eye fields, intraparietal sulcus) may play a less direct role (110). The exact balance of symptoms stemming from ADHD and emotion dysregulation in an individual may depend on the degree to which each neural network or level is compromised. We predict that dysfunction at the cortical nexus between cognition and emotion (the medial and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex) is strongly associated with symptoms of both ADHD and emotion dysregulation. If, however, an individual has dysfunction that is more focused in higher, more lateral prefrontal/parietal cortical regions, then in that individual symptoms of ADHD such as inattention would predominate over emotion dysregulation. Conversely, an individual with predominantly (para)limbic dysfunction may exhibit mainly symptoms stemming from emotion dysregulation.

Etiological factors.

It has been proposed that the combination of ADHD and emotion dysregulation defines a distinct genetic group. In support of this view, one group found that the siblings of probands with both ADHD and emotion dysregulation also had significantly elevated rates of this combination, although this has not been replicated (52, 111, 112). The Child Behavior Checklist-defined dysregulation profile is highly heritable (67%) (113), and studies have suggested candidate genes (114).
Among possible environmental factors, high levels of parental criticism and hostility have been linked both with the development of conduct problems in children with ADHD and with the development of childhood ADHD in preschoolers with behavioral problems (115, 116). A plausible hypothesis is that failures of parental emotion regulation, reflected by high expressed hostility, contribute to the development of emotion dysregulation in children with ADHD.

Treatment

The management of emotion dysregulation in ADHD presents formidable therapeutic challenges, partly because clinical trials in ADHD either fail to assess change in emotion regulation or do so as a secondary outcome measure. Psychostimulants are highly effective in treating oppositional defiant disorder comorbid with ADHD (meta-analyses are available at http://www.nice.org.uk/CG72). However, evidence for psychostimulant efficacy in treating emotion dysregulation in ADHD is more limited (Table 3). A review found that two randomized placebo-controlled studies in children with ADHD reported that psychostimulants reduced emotional lability and irritability (127). In adults, several studies found that the beneficial effects of psychostimulants on emotion dysregulation parallel the improvement seen in hyperactivity and impulsivity. However, two randomized controlled trials comparing amphetamine and placebo found no beneficial medication impact on a broad range of emotional problems, and some studies have found that amphetamine preparations increase irritability and lability (127).
TABLE 3. Randomized Controlled Treatment Studies in Children and Adults With ADHD in Which Change in Measures Reflecting Emotion Dysregulation Was Measureda
StudyParticipantsMeasuresResults
Children
Childress et al. (117)Lisdexamphetamine versus placebo for 4 weeks, N=283; at baseline 179 had prominent emotional labilityConners Parent Rating Scale of emotional lability (angry/resentful, losing temper, and irritability); prominent emotional lability defined as having at least one symptom “pretty much” or “very much”For those with prominent emotional lability, medication was associated with a significant reduction in emotional symptoms; no change in emotionality seen in those with low emotional lability
Ahmann et al. (118)Crossover design; treated with placebo or low-dosage (0.3 mg/kg) and higher-dosage (0.5 mg/kg) methylphenidate; N=234Side effect questionnaire including items on dysthymia, euphoria, irritability, and anxietyDecreased irritability on methylphenidate (odds ratio=0.33, 95% CI=0.18–0.61)
Gillberg et al. (119)Amphetamine versus placebo for 6 months, N=56Side effect questionnaire including items on dysthymia, euphoria, irritability, and anxietyNo differences between treated and placebo groups
Kratochvil et al. (120)Atomoxetine versus placebo, for 8 weeks, N=179Emotion and Expression Scale for ChildrenNo difference between atomoxetine and placebo
Coghill et al. (121)Crossover methylphenidate 0.3 mg/kg and 0.6 mg/kg versus placebo for 12 weeks, N=75Conners’ emotional lability subscaleSignificant reduction in emotional lability for both low-dosage (parent-report effect size, 0.46; teacher-report effect size, 0.45) and high-dosage methylphenidate (parent-report effect size, 0.42; teacher-report effect size, 0.79)
Herbert et al. (122)Randomized to waiting list or parent training to boost child’s emotion regulation and socialization, N=31Emotion Regulation ChecklistParent training linked with moderate reduction of child’s emotional lability (effect size, 0.27–0.45).
Webster-Stratton et al. (123)Randomized to waiting list or parenting program boosting positive and consistent parenting style, N=99Emotion regulation scaleModerate effect of intervention on emotion regulation (effect size, 0.25)
Adults
Reimherr et al. (60)Crossover trial of extended-release methylphenidate versus placebo (4 weeks each arm), N=47Wender-Reimherr adult ADHD scale, emotion dysregulation itemsDecrease in emotion dysregulation on methylphenidate (effect size, 0.7)
Reimherr et al. (59)Post hoc analyses of trials comparing atomoxetine and placebo; ADHD only, N=359; ADHD and emotion dysregulation, N=170Wender-Reimherr adult ADHD scale, emotion dysregulation itemsDecrease in emotion dysregulation on atomoxetine (effect size, 0.66)
Marchant et al. (124)Crossover trial of transdermal methylphenidate versus placebo (4 weeks each arm); ADHD alone, N=21; ADHD and emotion dysregulation, N=28; ADHD and oppositional defiant disorder, N=9; ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder, and emotion dysregulation, N=32Wender-Reimherr adult ADHD scale, emotion dysregulation itemsAll groups showed benefit on psychostimulants; trend for those with emotion dysregulation to improve most
Rösler et al. (125)Methylphenidate versus placebo with 24-week double-blind phase, N=363Wender-Reimherr adult ADHD scale, emotion dysregulation itemsMethylphenidate reduced emotional lability (effect size, 0.28–0.4)
Emilsson et al. (126)Cognitive behavioral therapy and medication versus medication alone, N=54Self-report scale of emotional controlCombination group did not show significantly better emotional control at end of intervention but did 3 months following intervention (d=1.12)
a
All medication studies were randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind trials.
Among individuals with ADHD, psychostimulants also improve emotion recognition (91) and normalize both the startle modulation by affective stimuli (93) and performance on the emotional Stroop task (89). This behavioral normalization is accompanied by normalization of underlying neural activity (89, 91, 93).
Among the non-stimulant treatments, improvement of emotion regulation on atomoxetine paralleled improvement in core symptoms of ADHD among adults (59). Mood stabilizers have yielded mixed results. A trial of lithium for children with severe mood dysregulation, most of whom had ADHD, was negative (128). However, a comparison of behavioral therapy combined with either divalproex or stimulants found that the use of divalproex was more efficacious in children with severe aggressive behavior, most of whom also had ADHD (129). Another study found that among 30 children with ADHD whose aggression did not respond to open-label psychostimulant treatment and behavioral therapy, the addition of divalproex resulted in significantly higher rates of remission compared with placebo (130). The use of atypical antipsychotic medications for the combination of ADHD, emotion dysregulation, and aggression still lacks a clear evidence base.
While cognitive and behavioral psychotherapies have a limited impact on core symptoms of ADHD, there is preliminary evidence that interventions that specifically target emotion dysregulation are efficacious (131, 132). We consider such interventions to be a promising future direction for research.
The current literature suggests the following treatment approach. Psychostimulant treatment of the core symptoms of ADHD is often linked to a beneficial effect on emotion dysregulation and should be considered first-line treatment. Atomoxetine also appears effective for symptoms of ADHD and emotion dysregulation. Use of adjunctive behavioral modification in children is reasonable, as this combination is effective in those with mixed internalizing and externalizing symptoms, many of whom have emotional dysregulation (51, 133). Group-based psychotherapy in adults with ADHD to bolster emotion regulation skills shows promise but requires replication (132). Lacking an evidence base for second-line pharmacological approaches to emotion dysregulation in ADHD, treatment will be guided largely by the presence of comorbid disorders. For example, for patients with ADHD and depression, in whom emotion dysregulation is often prominent, use of serotonin reuptake inhibitors combined with psychostimulants is reasonable (134).

Conceptual Models

Three models have been proposed, whose proponents can be characterized as “lumpers,” who view emotion dysregulation as an integral component of ADHD; “splitters,” who view the combination as defining a distinct entity; and “diplomats,” who view symptoms of ADHD and emotion dysregulation as correlated but ultimately dissociable dimensions (Table 4). The current evidence is insufficient to choose decisively among these models, partly because few studies have been designed to address specifically the question of why emotion dysregulation is so prominent in ADHD. However, this framework generates testable hypotheses that can stimulate future research.
TABLE 4. Three Models to Explain the Overlap Between ADHD and Emotion Dysregulation
 PhenomenologyPathophysiology 
ModelCorrelations Between ADHD and Emotion DysregulationClinical CoursePsychological BasisNeural BasisGeneticTreatment
Emotion dysregulation is integral to ADHDExtremely highYoked clinical courses for symptoms of ADHD and emotion dysregulationDeficits in behavioral inhibition and working memory mediate both core ADHD symptoms and emotion dysregulationAnomalies confined to fronto-striatal-cerebellar circuitsSame genetic basis for ADHD with emotion dysregulation and ADHD aloneTreatments that improve ADHD will improve emotion dysregulation
Combined ADHD and emotion dysregulation defines a distinct entityADHD subgroup exists that is high on both symptom domainsDistinct clinical course for ADHD with emotion dysregulation and ADHD aloneDistinct cognitive deficits in ADHD with emotion dysregulation and ADHD aloneDistinct neural bases for ADHD with emotion dysregulation and ADHD aloneDistinct genetic bases for ADHD with emotion dysregulation and ADHD aloneExisting treatments for ADHD may be less effective for ADHD with emotion dysregulation
Symptoms of ADHD and emotion dysregulation are correlated but distinct dimensionsModestSimilar but dissociable clinical courses for symptoms of ADHD and emotion dysregulationDeficits in emotion processing mediate dysregulation and correlate with deficits mediating core ADHD symptomsAnomalies extend beyond fronto-striato-cerebellar circuits to (para)limbic regionsSome genes shared between ADHD alone and ADHD with emotion dysregulationTreating “core” ADHD symptoms benefits emotion dysregulation, but separate treatment may also be needed
The first model, which harks back to earlier conceptualizations of ADHD, posits that emotion dysregulation is a core defining feature of ADHD that is as central to the disorder as hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention (135). Emotion dysregulation is seen as an expression of the same neurocognitive deficits that underpin other symptoms of ADHD, and Occam’s razor dictates that it is unnecessary to invoke additional emotion processing deficits. The model is parsimonious and recognizes the close associations between cognitive and emotional regulation systems. However, as noted above, the overlap between ADHD and emotion dysregulation is far from complete: many ADHD patients do not exhibit impairing levels of emotion dysregulation (55%–75% of children and 30%–70% of adults with ADHD). Additionally, the evidence for widespread (para)limbic dysfunction in ADHD and associated deficits in emotional processes might argue against a reductionist model of emotion dysregulation in ADHD as another expression of purely cortico-striatal-cerebellar dysfunction. This model also predicts that treatments that ameliorate core symptoms would have an almost equal impact on emotion dysregulation, which seems to occur in adulthood but does so less clearly in childhood.
The second model holds that the combination of ADHD and emotion dysregulation defines a distinct entity (111, 112). This model has been generated largely on the basis of genetic findings of familial cosegregation of ADHD and emotion dysregulation, although evidence on this point is mixed (52, 111). This model could imply both a distinct neurocognitive etiology and a distinct clinical course for those with the combination of ADHD and emotion dysregulation, a possibility that warrants further testing.
The third model holds that symptoms of ADHD and emotion dysregulation are distinct but correlated dimensions, each underpinned by partly overlapping but dissociable neurocognitive deficits. This model has much in common with the concept of multiple but overlapping pathways to ADHD (103, 110). It is supported by the significant but modest correlations between symptoms of ADHD and emotion dysregulation reviewed earlier; the symptom domains commonly coexist but are far from completely overlapping. Similarly, modest correlations have been reported between deficits in emotional processes—such as deficits in emotion recognition and frustration tolerance—and the executive dysfunction often held to be a core feature of the disorder (35, 37). Longitudinal data reviewed earlier also suggest modest links between the course of ADHD symptoms and emotion dysregulation in early childhood, and perhaps in adulthood, consistent with a model of correlated but distinct symptom dimensions.

Future Research Directions

Phenomenology and Pathophysiology

Refinement of the phenotype is needed, as emotion dysregulation in individuals with ADHD is likely to have a number of clinically important components, such as irritability and mood lability (4, 51). It will be important to operationalize each component, develop consensus measurement techniques, and conduct longitudinal studies to define how the developmental trajectories of the components interact with each other and with the dimensions of ADHD. Pathophysiological studies should include individuals lying along the spectrum of emotion regulation abilities, but perhaps oversample those most in clinical need, lying at the extreme of dysregulation. Such work would allow direct links to be made between emotion dysregulation in ADHD and the underlying neural anomalies—a link that has been made in relatively few studies.
Functional imaging studies should include a broad range of tasks of emotion regulation, defining the neural bases of the ability to reinterpret the meaning of emotional stimuli, the adaptive suppression of ongoing emotional responses, and the ability to employ strategies such as distancing oneself from emotionally arousing materials (77). We predict that emotionally dysregulated individuals with ADHD would lose the coordinated increase in medial prefrontal/anterior cingulate cortex activation and altered amygdala activation that underpins many forms of emotion regulation.
To what extent do individuals with ADHD develop emotion dysregulation for reasons different from those of individuals with other disorders? Could ADHD-specific symptoms or cognitive aberrations be related to emotion dysregulation? “Mind wandering” is one candidate cognitive mechanism. It is typically measured as interference in tasks of cognitive control and appears to be related to a failure to deactivate the so-called default mode network of the brain, a deficit also found in ADHD (136). Notably, mind wandering appears to lead to transient dysphoric mood and vice versa (137). Testing the links between attentional lapses and emotion dysregulation and the possible mediating role of the default mode network may be a promising research avenue.
There is evidence in ADHD of an altered structural and functional maturation of the prefrontal cortical regions that support top-down emotion regulation (138). Could disrupted developmental trajectories be particularly pronounced among those with both ADHD and impaired emotion regulation? Our model predicts a disruption of white matter tracts such as the uncinate fasciculus that connect limbic regions, including the amygdala, hippocampus, and orbitofrontal cortex; these tracts can be assessed using diffusion tensor imaging.
Behavioral genetic data on twins could parse out the degree to which ADHD and emotion dysregulation share genetic or environmental risk factors. This could be achieved by reanalysis of existing data sets that include measures of irritability and other relevant traits. Studies of environmental risk factors have focused on familial characteristics but could also define the characteristics of a child’s peer group that confer vulnerability to emotion dysregulation.

Treatment

Is a two-pronged pharmacological approach targeting both the symptoms of ADHD and those of emotion dysregulation more effective than use of psychostimulants alone? This question is being examined in children with severe mood dysregulation (most of whom have ADHD) in trials that use both psychostimulants and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) (clinicaltrials.gov identifiers NCT00794040, NCT01714310). The use of SSRIs is grounded in preclinical studies showing that the modulation of serotonergic tone affects the processing of emotionally charged stimuli and clinical studies showing efficacy in promoting emotion regulation in other disorders (139).
Other agents show promise. Postsynaptic alpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists such as guanfacine treat core symptoms of ADHD and oppositionality (140). In healthy adults, guanfacine reverses the bias to respond less accurately to negative compared with positive emotional stimuli, partly by boosting activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (141). Given the interactions between the lateral prefrontal cortex and the ventral/medial prefrontal cortical regions linked to emotion regulation, guanfacine emerges as a potential emotion regulator in ADHD. Modafinil, which inhibits dopamine and norepinephrine transporters and decreases GABA, also shows promise as a treatment for ADHD (142). In healthy adults, modafinil decreases amygdala activation during the viewing of fearful stimuli and boosts prefrontal cortical activation during executive functions (143). Again, this profile points to a possible benefit for ADHD and associated emotion dysregulation. Dietary interventions can be considered, as there appears to be benefit from omega-3 fatty acid supplementation in ADHD (131). Given that low levels of omega-3 fatty acids are associated with electrophysiological anomalies during emotion processing in ADHD (144), might emotion dysregulation in ADHD also benefit from supplementation?
Which psychotherapies are promising? Cognitive therapy can help individuals with ADHD recognize and label emotions accurately, challenge emotions that are not context appropriate, and cope with intense negative emotional reactions (132). These skills have been augmented with mindfulness training that promotes a nonjudgmental, present-centered focused awareness of emotions. This approach, derived partly from dialectical behavior therapy for disorders with prominent emotion dysregulation, such as borderline personality disorder, is currently being assessed in adults with ADHD (73). Improving executive functions such as working memory and planning abilities helps core ADHD symptoms in adults, but future studies should also ask if these cognitive interventions also improve emotion regulation (132). Similarly, it has been argued that parent-led games can boost a preschooler’s executive skills and might prevent later ADHD (145, 146). Might such early intervention also promote emotion regulation?
What of interventions that target not just the individual with ADHD but also the individual’s social context? For example, there is a strong rationale for family-based interventions to decrease negative family dynamics and thus perhaps enhance emotion regulation in both the parent and the child with ADHD (115). A novel approach leverages a child’s peer group as a therapeutic ally (147). Children with ADHD often form cliques with disruptive others, and classroom interventions might promote alliances with less disruptive children who can perhaps better model emotion regulation.

Conclusions

Since Still described the “morbid excitability” of children with ADHD (148), the presence of emotion dysregulation in ADHD has been well recognized. Recent advances in the behavioral, neuroimaging, and genomic sciences hold the promise that our renewed focus on this overlap will result in an understanding of the underlying pathophysiological mechanisms and stimulate novel treatment approaches.

Supplementary Material

File (appi.focus.140102.ds001.pdf)

References

1.
Clements SD: Minimal Brain Dysfunction in Children: Terminology and Identification: Phase One of a Three-Phase Project. Washington, DC, US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1966
2.
Thompson RA: Emotion Regulation: A Theme in Search of Definition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, vol 59, issue 2/3, Feb 1994, pp 25–52
3.
Leibenluft E: Severe mood dysregulation, irritability, and the diagnostic boundaries of bipolar disorder in youths. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168:129–142
4.
Stringaris A: Irritability in children and adolescents: a challenge for DSM-5. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011; 20:61–66
5.
Dodge KA, Pepler D, Rubin K: The structure and function of reactive and proactive aggression, in The Development and Treatment of Childhood Aggression. Edited by Pepler DJ, Rubin KH. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum, 1991, pp 201–218
6.
Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA: ASEBA School Age Forms and Profiles. Burlington, Vt, ASEBA, 2001
7.
Ambrosini PJ, Bennett DS, Elia J: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder characteristics, II: clinical correlates of irritable mood. J Affect Disord 2013; 145:70–76
8.
Stringaris A, Goodman R: Three dimensions of oppositionality in youth. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009; 50:216–223
9.
Abikoff HB, Jensen PS, Arnold LL, Hoza B, Hechtman L, Pollack S, Martin D, Alvir J, March JS, Hinshaw S, Vitiello B, Newcorn J, Greiner A, Cantwell DP, Conners CK, Elliott G, Greenhill LL, Kraemer H, Pelham WE, Severe JB, Swanson JM, Wells K, Wigal T: Observed classroom behavior of children with ADHD: relationship to gender and comorbidity. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2002; 30:349–359
10.
Hoza B, Murray-Close D, Arnold LE, Hinshaw SP, Hechtman L; MTA Cooperative Group: Time-dependent changes in positively biased self-perceptions of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a developmental psychopathology perspective. Dev Psychopathol 2010; 22:375–390
11.
Mikami AY, Ransone ML, Calhoun CD: Influence of anxiety on the social functioning of children with and without ADHD. J Atten Disord 2011; 15:473–484
12.
Cadesky EB, Mota VL, Schachar RJ: Beyond words: how do children with ADHD and/or conduct problems process nonverbal information about affect? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39:1160–1167
13.
McQuade JD, Vaughn AJ, Hoza B, Murray-Close D, Molina BS, Arnold LE, Hechtman L: Perceived social acceptance and peer status differentially predict adjustment in youth with and without ADHD. J Atten Disord (Epub ahead of print, April 3, 2012)
14.
Buhrmester D, Whalen CK, Henker B, MacDonald V, Hinshaw SP: Prosocial behavior in hyperactive boys: effects of stimulant medication and comparison with normal boys. J Abnorm Child Psychol 1992; 20:103–121
15.
Greene RW, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Ouellette CA, Penn C, Griffin SM: Toward a new psychometric definition of social disability in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996; 35:571–578
16.
Matthys W, Cuperus JM, Van Engeland H: Deficient social problem-solving in boys with ODD/CD, with ADHD, and with both disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999; 38:311–321
17.
Ohan JL, Johnston C: Positive illusions of social competence in girls with and without ADHD. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2011; 39:527–539
18.
Zalecki CA, Hinshaw SP: Overt and relational aggression in girls with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2004; 33:125–137
19.
King S, Waschbusch DA, Pelham WE, Frankland BW, Corkum PV, Jacques S: Subtypes of aggression in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: medication effects and comparison with typical children. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2009; 38:619–629
20.
Waschbusch DA, Pelham WE, Jennings JR, Greiner AR, Tarter RE, Moss HB: Reactive aggression in boys with disruptive behavior disorders: behavior, physiology, and affect. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2002; 30:641–656
21.
Boakes J, Chapman E, Houghton S, West J: Facial affect interpretation in boys with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Neuropsychol 2008; 14:82–96
22.
Sinzig J, Morsch D, Lehmkuhl G: Do hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention have an impact on the ability of facial affect recognition in children with autism and ADHD? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2008; 17:63–72
23.
Rapport LJ, Friedman SR, Tzelepis A, Van Voorhis A: Experienced emotion and affect recognition in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology 2002; 16:102–110
24.
Herpertz SC, Huebner T, Marx I, Vloet TD, Fink GR, Stoecker T, Shah NJ, Konrad K, Herpertz-Dahlmann B: Emotional processing in male adolescents with childhood-onset conduct disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2008; 49:781–791
25.
Dyck MJ, Ferguson K, Shochet IM: Do autism spectrum disorders differ from each other and from non-spectrum disorders on emotion recognition tests? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001; 10:105–116
26.
Pelc K, Kornreich C, Foisy M-L, Dan B: Recognition of emotional facial expressions in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pediatr Neurol 2006; 35:93–97
27.
Shin DW, Lee SJ, Kim BJ, Park Y, Lim SW: Visual attention deficits contribute to impaired facial emotion recognition in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropediatrics 2008; 39:323–327
28.
Da Fonseca D, Seguier V, Santos A, Poinso F, Deruelle C: Emotion understanding in children with ADHD. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2009; 40:111–121
29.
Malisza KL, Clancy C, Shiloff D, Holden J, Jones C, Paulson K, Yu DC, Summers R, Chudley AE: Functional magnetic resonance imaging of facial information processing in children with autistic disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and typically developing controls. Int J Adolesc Med Health 2011; 23:269–277
30.
Corbett B, Glidden H: Processing affective stimuli in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Child Neuropsychol 2000; 6:144–155
31.
Yuill N, Lyon J: Selective difficulty in recognising facial expressions of emotion in boys with ADHD: general performance impairments or specific problems in social cognition? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2007; 16:398–404
32.
Seymour KE, Pescosolido MF, Reidy BL, Galvan T, Kim KL, Young M, Dickstein DP: Emotional face identification in youths with primary bipolar disorder or primary attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2013; 52:537–46.e3
33.
Downs A, Smith T: Emotional understanding, cooperation, and social behavior in high-functioning children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2004; 34:625–635
34.
Greenbaum RL, Stevens SA, Nash K, Koren G, Rovet J: Social cognitive and emotion processing abilities of children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: a comparison with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2009; 33:1656–1670
35.
Sjöwall D, Roth L, Lindqvist S, Thorell LB: Multiple deficits in ADHD: executive dysfunction, delay aversion, reaction time variability, and emotional deficits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2013; 54:619–627
36.
Miller M, Hanford RB, Fassbender C, Duke M, Schweitzer JB: Affect recognition in adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord 2011; 15:452–460
37.
Banaschewski T, Jennen-Steinmetz C, Brandeis D, Buitelaar JK, Kuntsi J, Poustka L, Sergeant JA, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Frazier-Wood AC, Albrecht B, Chen W, Uebel H, Schlotz W, van der Meere JJ, Gill M, Manor I, Miranda A, Mulas F, Oades RD, Roeyers H, Rothenberger A, Steinhausen HC, Faraone SV, Asherson P: Neuropsychological correlates of emotional lability in children with ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2012; 53:1139–1148
38.
Plichta MM, Vasic N, Wolf RC, Lesch K-P, Brummer D, Jacob C, Fallgatter AJ, Grön G: Neural hyporesponsiveness and hyperresponsiveness during immediate and delayed reward processing in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2009; 65:7–14
39.
Antrop I, Stock P, Verté S, Wiersema JR, Baeyens D, Roeyers H: ADHD and delay aversion: the influence of non-temporal stimulation on choice for delayed rewards. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2006; 47:1152–1158
40.
Bidwell LC, Willcutt EG, Defries JC, Pennington BF: Testing for neuropsychological endophenotypes in siblings discordant for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:991–998
41.
Bitsakou P, Psychogiou L, Thompson M, Sonuga-Barke EJ: Delay aversion in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an empirical investigation of the broader phenotype. Neuropsychologia 2009; 47:446–456
42.
Dalen L, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Hall M, Remington B: Inhibitory deficits, delay aversion, and preschool AD/HD: implications for the dual pathway model. Neural Plast 2004; 11:1–11
43.
Karalunas SL, Huang-Pollock CL: Examining relationships between executive functioning and delay aversion in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2011; 40:837–847
44.
Kuntsi J, Oosterlaan J, Stevenson J: Psychological mechanisms in hyperactivity, I: response inhibition deficit, working memory impairment, delay aversion, or something else? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001; 42:199–210
45.
Marco R, Miranda A, Schlotz W, Melia A, Mulligan A, Müller U, Andreou P, Butler L, Christiansen H, Gabriels I, Medad S, Albrecht B, Uebel H, Asherson P, Banaschewski T, Gill M, Kuntsi J, Mulas F, Oades R, Roeyers H, Steinhausen HC, Rothenberger A, Faraone SV, Sonuga-Barke EJ: Delay and reward choice in ADHD: an experimental test of the role of delay aversion. Neuropsychology 2009; 23:367–380
46.
Marx I, Hübner T, Herpertz SC, Berger C, Reuter E, Kircher T, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K: Cross-sectional evaluation of cognitive functioning in children, adolescents, and young adults with ADHD. J Neural Transm 2010; 117:403–419
47.
Solanto MV, Gilbert SN, Raj A, Zhu J, Pope-Boyd S, Stepak B, Vail L, Newcorn JH: Neurocognitive functioning in AD/HD, predominantly inattentive and combined subtypes. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2007; 35:729–744
48.
Solanto MV, Abikoff H, Sonuga-Barke E, Schachar R, Logan GD, Wigal T, Hechtman L, Hinshaw S, Turkel E: The ecological validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures of impulsivity in AD/HD: a supplement to the NIMH Multimodal Treatment Study of AD/HD. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2001; 29:215–228
49.
Vloet TD, Marx I, Kahraman-Lanzerath B, Zepf FD, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Konrad K: Neurocognitive performance in children with ADHD and OCD. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2010; 38:961–969
50.
Yang BR, Chan RC, Gracia N, Cao XY, Zou XB, Jing J, Mai JN, Li J, Shum D: Cool and hot executive functions in medication-naive attention deficit hyperactivity disorder children. Psychol Med 2011; 41:2593–2602
51.
Stringaris A, Goodman R: Mood lability and psychopathology in youth. Psychol Med 2009; 39:1237–1245
52.
Sobanski E, Banaschewski T, Asherson P, Buitelaar J, Chen W, Franke B, Holtmann M, Krumm B, Sergeant J, Sonuga-Barke E, Stringaris A, Taylor E, Anney R, Ebstein RP, Gill M, Miranda A, Mulas F, Oades RD, Roeyers H, Rothenberger A, Steinhausen HC, Faraone SV: Emotional lability in children and adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): clinical correlates and familial prevalence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2010; 51:915–923
53.
Anastopoulos AD, Smith TF, Garrett ME, Morrissey-Kane E, Schatz NK, Sommer JL, Kollins SH, Ashley-Koch A: Self-regulation of emotion, functional impairment, and comorbidity among children with AD/HD. J Atten Disord 2011; 15:583–592
54.
Spencer TJ, Faraone SV, Surman CB, Petty C, Clarke A, Batchelder H, Wozniak J, Biederman J: Toward defining deficient emotional self-regulation in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder using the Child Behavior Checklist: a controlled study. Postgrad Med 2011; 123:50–59
55.
Strine TW, Lesesne CA, Okoro CA, McGuire LC, Chapman DP, Balluz LS, Mokdad AH: Emotional and behavioral difficulties and impairments in everyday functioning among children with a history of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Prev Chronic Dis 2006; 3:A52
56.
Becker A, Steinhausen HC, Baldursson G, Dalsgaard S, Lorenzo MJ, Ralston SJ, Döpfner M, Rothenberger A; ADORE Study Group: Psychopathological screening of children with ADHD: Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in a pan-European study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2006; 15(suppl 1):I56–I62
57.
Able SL, Johnston JA, Adler LA, Swindle RW: Functional and psychosocial impairment in adults with undiagnosed ADHD. Psychol Med 2007; 37:97–107
58.
Barkley RA, Fischer M: The unique contribution of emotional impulsiveness to impairment in major life activities in hyperactive children as adults. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010; 49:503–513
59.
Reimherr FW, Marchant BK, Strong RE, Hedges DW, Adler L, Spencer TJ, West SA, Soni P: Emotional dysregulation in adult ADHD and response to atomoxetine. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58:125–131
60.
Reimherr FW, Williams ED, Strong RE, Mestas R, Soni P, Marchant BK: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study of osmotic release oral system methylphenidate in adults with ADHD with assessment of oppositional and emotional dimensions of the disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 2007; 68:93–101
61.
Surman CB, Biederman J, Spencer T, Miller CA, McDermott KM, Faraone SV: Understanding deficient emotional self-regulation in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a controlled study. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord 2013; 5:273–281
62.
Althoff RR, Copeland WE, Stanger C, Derks EM, Todd RD, Neuman RJ, Van Beijsterveldt TC, Boomsma DI, Hudziak JJ: The latent class structure of ADHD is stable across informants. Twin Res Hum Genet 2006; 9:507–522
63.
Stringaris A, Goodman R: Longitudinal outcome of youth oppositionality: irritable, headstrong, and hurtful behaviors have distinctive predictions. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009; 48:404–412
64.
Goldsmith H, Lemery K, Essex M: Roles for temperament in the liability to psychopathology in childhood, in Behavior Genetics Principles: Perspectives in Development, Personality, and Psychopathology. Edited by DiLalla LF. Washington, DC, American Psychological Association Press, 2004, pp 9–19
65.
Bates JE, Pettit GS, Dodge KA, Ridge B: Interaction of temperamental resistance to control and restrictive parenting in the development of externalizing behavior. Dev Psychol 1998; 34:982–995
66.
Carlson EA, Jacobvitz D, Sroufe LA: A developmental investigation of inattentiveness and hyperactivity. Child Dev 1995; 66:37–54
67.
Olson SL, Bates JE, Sandy JM, Schilling EM: Early developmental precursors of impulsive and inattentive behavior: from infancy to middle childhood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2002; 43:435–447
68.
Stringaris A, Maughan B, Goodman R: What’s in a disruptive disorder? Temperamental antecedents of oppositional defiant disorder: findings from the Avon Longitudinal Study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010; 49:474–483
69.
Sanson A, Smart D, Prior M, Oberklaid F: Precursors of hyperactivity and aggression. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1993; 32:1207–1216
70.
Klein RG, Mannuzza S, Olazagasti MA, Roizen E, Hutchison JA, Lashua EC, Castellanos FX: Clinical and functional outcome of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder 33 years later. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2012; 69:1295–1303
71.
Biederman J, Spencer TJ, Petty C, Hyder LL, O’Connor KB, Surman CB, Faraone SV: Longitudinal course of deficient emotional self-regulation CBCL profile in youth with ADHD: prospective controlled study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2012; 8:267–276
72.
Althoff RR, Verhulst FC, Rettew DC, Hudziak JJ, van der Ende J: Adult outcomes of childhood dysregulation: a 14-year follow-up study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010; 49:1105–1116
73.
Wender PH, Reimherr FW, Wood DR: Attention deficit disorder (“minimal brain dysfunction”) in adults: a replication study of diagnosis and drug treatment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1981; 38:449–456
74.
Riley AW, Spiel G, Coghill D, Döpfner M, Falissard B, Lorenzo MJ, Preuss U, Ralston SJ; ADORE Study Group: Factors related to health-related quality of life (HRQoL) among children with ADHD in Europe at entry into treatment. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2006; 15(suppl 1):I38–I45
75.
Wehmeier PM, Schacht A, Barkley RA: Social and emotional impairment in children and adolescents with ADHD and the impact on quality of life. J Adolesc Health 2010; 46:209–217
76.
Biederman J, Petty C, Monuteaux MC, Evans M, Parcell T, Faraone SV, Wozniak J: The Child Behavior Checklist–Pediatric Bipolar Disorder profile predicts a subsequent diagnosis of bipolar disorder and associated impairments in ADHD youth growing up: a longitudinal analysis. J Clin Psychiatry 2009; 70:732–740
77.
Phillips ML, Ladouceur CD, Drevets WC: A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry 2008; 13:829-, 833–857
78.
Ochsner KN, Gross JJ: The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci 2005; 9:242–249
79.
Brotman MA, Rich BA, Guyer AE, Lunsford JR, Horsey SE, Reising MM, Thomas LA, Fromm SJ, Towbin K, Pine DS, Leibenluft E: Amygdala activation during emotion processing of neutral faces in children with severe mood dysregulation versus ADHD or bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2010; 167:61–69
80.
Marsh AA, Finger EC, Mitchell DG, Reid ME, Sims C, Kosson DS, Towbin KE, Leibenluft E, Pine DS, Blair RJ: Reduced amygdala response to fearful expressions in children and adolescents with callous-unemotional traits and disruptive behavior disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:712–720
81.
Posner J, Nagel BJ, Maia TV, Mechling A, Oh M, Wang Z, Peterson BS: Abnormal amygdalar activation and connectivity in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011; 50:828–837.e3
82.
Schlochtermeier L, Stoy M, Schlagenhauf F, Wrase J, Park SQ, Friedel E, Huss M, Lehmkuhl U, Heinz A, Ströhle A: Childhood methylphenidate treatment of ADHD and response to affective stimuli. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2011; 21:646–654
83.
Ströhle A, Stoy M, Wrase J, Schwarzer S, Schlagenhauf F, Huss M, Hein J, Nedderhut A, Neumann B, Gregor A, Juckel G, Knutson B, Lehmkuhl U, Bauer M, Heinz A: Reward anticipation and outcomes in adult males with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuroimage 2008; 39:966–972
84.
Scheres A, Milham MP, Knutson B, Castellanos FX: Ventral striatal hyporesponsiveness during reward anticipation in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 61:720–724
85.
Stoy M, Schlagenhauf F, Schlochtermeier L, Wrase J, Knutson B, Lehmkuhl U, Huss M, Heinz A, Ströhle A: Reward processing in male adults with childhood ADHD: a comparison between drug-naive and methylphenidate-treated subjects. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011; 215:467–481
86.
Rubia K, Halari R, Cubillo A, Mohammad AM, Brammer M, Taylor E: Methylphenidate normalises activation and functional connectivity deficits in attention and motivation networks in medication-naive children with ADHD during a rewarded continuous performance task. Neuropharmacology 2009; 57:640–652
87.
Passarotti AM, Sweeney JA, Pavuluri MN: Emotion processing influences working memory circuits in pediatric bipolar disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2010; 49:1064–1080
88.
Passarotti AM, Sweeney JA, Pavuluri MN: Differential engagement of cognitive and affective neural systems in pediatric bipolar disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2010; 16:106–117
89.
Posner J, Maia TV, Fair D, Peterson BS, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Nagel BJ: The attenuation of dysfunctional emotional processing with stimulant medication: an fMRI study of adolescents with ADHD. Psychiatry Res 2011; 193:151–160
90.
Skirrow C, Asherson P: Emotional lability, comorbidity, and impairment in adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Affect Disord 2012; 147:80–86
91.
Williams LM, Hermens DF, Palmer D, Kohn M, Clarke S, Keage H, Clark CR, Gordon E: Misinterpreting emotional expressions in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence for a neural marker and stimulant effects. Biol Psychiatry 2008; 63:917–926
92.
Du J, Li J, Wang Y, Jiang Q, Livesley WJ, Jang KL, Wang K, Wang W: Event-related potentials in adolescents with combined ADHD and CD disorder: a single stimulus paradigm. Brain Cogn 2006; 60:70–75
93.
Conzelmann A, Woidich E, Mucha RF, Weyers P, Jacob CP, Lesch K-P, Pauli P: Methylphenidate normalizes emotional processing in adult patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: preliminary findings. Brain Res 2011; 1381:159–166
94.
Campos JJ, Frankel CB, Camras L: On the nature of emotion regulation. Child Dev 2004; 75:377–394
95.
Penton-Voak IS, Thomas J, Gage SH, McMurran M, McDonald S, Munafò MR: Increasing recognition of happiness in ambiguous facial expressions reduces anger and aggressive behavior. Psychol Sci 2013; 24:688–697
96.
Sagvolden T, Johansen EB, Aase H, Russell VA: A dynamic developmental theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) predominantly hyperactive/impulsive and combined subtypes. Behav Brain Sci 2005; 28:397–419, discussion 419–468
97.
Sonuga-Barke EJ, Dalen L, Remington B: Do executive deficits and delay aversion make independent contributions to preschool attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2003; 42:1335–1342
98.
Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL: Emotion-related regulation: sharpening the definition. Child Dev 2004; 75:334–339
99.
Haber SN, Knutson B: The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010; 35:4–26
100.
Musser ED, Galloway-Long HS, Frick PJ, Nigg JT: Emotion regulation and heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2013; 52:163–171.e2
101.
Gross JJ: The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol 1998; 2:271
102.
Walcott CM, Landau S: The relation between disinhibition and emotion regulation in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2004; 33:772–782
103.
Nigg JT, Casey BJ: An integrative theory of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder based on the cognitive and affective neurosciences. Dev Psychopathol 2005; 17:785–806
104.
Plessen KJ, Bansal R, Zhu H, Whiteman R, Amat J, Quackenbush GA, Martin L, Durkin K, Blair C, Royal J, Hugdahl K, Peterson BS: Hippocampus and amygdala morphology in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006; 63:795–807
105.
O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C: Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2001; 4:95–102
106.
Overmeyer S, Bullmore ET, Suckling J, Simmons A, Williams SC, Santosh PJ, Taylor E: Distributed grey and white matter deficits in hyperkinetic disorder: MRI evidence for anatomical abnormality in an attentional network. Psychol Med 2001; 31:1425–1435
107.
Knutson B, Adams CM, Fong GW, Hommer D: Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci 2001; 21:RC159
108.
Tomasi D, Volkow ND: Abnormal functional connectivity in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2012; 71:443–450
109.
Costa Dias TG, Wilson VB, Bathula DR, Iyer SP, Mills KL, Thurlow BL, Stevens CA, Musser ED, Carpenter SD, Grayson DS, Mitchell SH, Nigg JT, Fair DA: Reward circuit connectivity relates to delay discounting in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2012; 23:33–45
110.
Castellanos FX, Proal E: Large-scale brain systems in ADHD: beyond the prefrontal-striatal model. Trends Cogn Sci 2012; 16:17–26
111.
Surman CB, Biederman J, Spencer T, Yorks D, Miller CA, Petty CR, Faraone SV: Deficient emotional self-regulation and adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a family risk analysis. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168:617–623
112.
Biederman J, Spencer T, Lomedico A, Day H, Petty CR, Faraone SV: Deficient emotional self-regulation and pediatric attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a family risk analysis. Psychol Med 2012; 42:639–646
113.
Hudziak JJ, Althoff RR, Derks EM, Faraone SV, Boomsma DI: Prevalence and genetic architecture of Child Behavior Checklist-juvenile bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58:562–568
114.
Mick E, McGough J, Loo S, Doyle AE, Wozniak J, Wilens TE, Smalley S, McCracken J, Biederman J, Faraone SV: Genome-wide association study of the Child Behavior Checklist dysregulation profile. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011; 50:807–817.e8
115.
Taylor E: Developmental neuropsychopathology of attention deficit and impulsiveness. Dev Psychopathol 1999; 11:607–628
116.
Peris TS, Baker BL: Applications of the expressed emotion construct to young children with externalizing behavior: stability and prediction over time. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000; 41:457–462
117.
Childress AC, Arnold V, Adeyi B, Dirks B, Babcock T, Scheckner B, Lasser R, Lopez FA: The effects of lisdexamfetamine dimesylate on emotional lability in children 6 to 12 years of age with ADHD in a double-blind placebo-controlled trial. J Atten Disord (Epub ahead of print, June 26, 2012)
118.
Ahmann PA, Waltonen SJ, Olson KA, Theye FW, Van Erem AJ, LaPlant RJ: Placebo-controlled evaluation of Ritalin side effects. Pediatrics 1993; 91:1101–1106
119.
Gillberg C, Melander H, von Knorring AL, Janols LO, Thernlund G, Hägglöf B, Eidevall-Wallin L, Gustafsson P, Kopp S: Long-term stimulant treatment of children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997; 54:857–864
120.
Kratochvil CJ, Faries D, Vaughan B, Perwien A, Busner J, Saylor K, Kaplan S, Buermeyer C, Swindle R: Emotional expression during attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders treatment: initial assessment of treatment effects. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2007; 17:51–62
121.
Coghill DR, Rhodes SM, Matthews K: The neuropsychological effects of chronic methylphenidate on drug-naive boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:954–962
122.
Herbert S, Harvey E, Roberts J, Wichowski K, Lugo-Candelas CI: A randomized controlled trial of a parent training and emotion socialization program for families of hyperactive preschool-aged children. Behav Ther 2013; 44:302–316
123.
Webster-Stratton CH, Reid MJ, Beauchaine T: Combining parent and child training for young children with ADHD. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2011; 40:191–203
124.
Marchant BK, Reimherr FW, Robison RJ, Olsen JL, Kondo DG: Methylphenidate transdermal system in adult ADHD and impact on emotional and oppositional symptoms. J Atten Disord 2011; 15:295–304
125.
Rösler M, Retz W, Fischer R, Ose C, Alm B, Deckert J, Philipsen A, Herpertz S, Ammer R: Twenty-four-week treatment with extended release methylphenidate improves emotional symptoms in adult ADHD. World J Biol Psychiatry 2010; 11:709–718
126.
Emilsson B, Gudjonsson G, Sigurdsson JF, Baldursson G, Einarsson E, Olafsdottir H, Young S: Cognitive behaviour therapy in medication-treated adults with ADHD and persistent symptoms: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry 2011; 11:116
127.
Manos MJ, Brams M, Childress AC, Findling RL, López FA, Jensen PS: Changes in emotions related to medication used to treat ADHD, part I: literature review. J Atten Disord 2011; 15:101–112
128.
Dickstein DP, Towbin KE, Van Der Veen JW, Rich BA, Brotman MA, Knopf L, Onelio L, Pine DS, Leibenluft E: Randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial of lithium in youths with severe mood dysregulation. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2009; 19:61–73
129.
Donovan SJ, Stewart JW, Nunes EV, Quitkin FM, Parides M, Daniel W, Susser E, Klein DF: Divalproex treatment for youth with explosive temper and mood lability: a double-blind, placebo-controlled crossover design. Am J Psychiatry 2000; 157:818–820
130.
Blader JC, Schooler NR, Jensen PS, Pliszka SR, Kafantaris V: Adjunctive divalproex versus placebo for children with ADHD and aggression refractory to stimulant monotherapy. Am J Psychiatry 2009; 166:1392–1401
131.
Sonuga-Barke EJ, Brandeis D, Cortese S, Daley D, Ferrin M, Holtmann M, Stevenson J, Danckaerts M, van der Oord S, Döpfner M, Dittmann RW, Simonoff E, Zuddas A, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, Coghill D, Hollis C, Konofal E, Lecendreux M, Wong IC, Sergeant J; European ADHD Guidelines Group: Nonpharmacological interventions for ADHD: systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of dietary and psychological treatments. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170:275–289
132.
Mongia M, Hechtman L: Cognitive behavior therapy for adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review of recent randomized controlled trials. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2012; 14:561–567
133.
Jensen PS, Hinshaw SP, Kraemer HC, Lenora N, Newcorn JH, Abikoff HB, March JS, Arnold LE, Cantwell DP, Conners CK, Elliott GR, Greenhill LL, Hechtman L, Hoza B, Pelham WE, Severe JB, Swanson JM, Wells KC, Wigal T, Vitiello B: ADHD comorbidity findings from the MTA study: comparing comorbid subgroups. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2001; 40:147–158
134.
Daviss WB: A review of co-morbid depression in pediatric ADHD: etiology, phenomenology, and treatment. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol 2008; 18:565–571
135.
Barkley RA, Murphy KR: Deficient emotional self-regulation in children and adults with attention deficit/hhyepractivity disorder. Journal of ADHD and Related Disorders 2010; 1:5–29
136.
Konrad K, Eickhoff SB: Is the ADHD brain wired differently? A review on structural and functional connectivity in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Hum Brain Mapp 2010; 31:904–916
137.
Killingsworth MA, Gilbert DT: A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science 2010; 330:932
138.
Shaw P, Malek M, Watson B, Sharp W, Evans A, Greenstein D: Development of cortical surface area and gyrification in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2012; 72:191–197
139.
Cools R, Roberts AC, Robbins TW: Serotoninergic regulation of emotional and behavioural control processes. Trends Cogn Sci 2008; 12:31–40
140.
Connor D, Findling R, Kollins S, Sallee F, López F, Lyne A, Tremblay G: Effects of guanfacine extended release on oppositional symptoms in children aged 6–12 years with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and oppositional symptoms: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. CNS Drugs 2010; 24:755–768
141.
Schulz KP, Clerkin SM, Fan J, Halperin JM, Newcorn JH: Guanfacine modulates the influence of emotional cues on prefrontal cortex activation for cognitive control. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2013; 226:261–271
142.
Arnold VK, Feifel D, Earl CQ, Yang R, Adler LA: A 9-week, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group, dose-finding study to evaluate the efficacy and safety of modafinil as treatment for adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord (Epub ahead of print, May 22, 2012)
143.
Rasetti R, Mattay VS, Stankevich B, Skjei K, Blasi G, Sambataro F, Arrillaga-Romany IC, Goldberg TE, Callicott JH, Apud JA, Weinberger DR: Modulatory effects of modafinil on neural circuits regulating emotion and cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010; 35:2101–2109
144.
Gow RV, Sumich A, Vallee-Tourangeau F, Crawford MA, Ghebremeskel K, Bueno AA, Hibbeln JR, Taylor E, Wilson DA, Rubia K: Omega-3 fatty acids are related to abnormal emotion processing in adolescent boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 2013; 88:419–429
145.
O’Neill S, Rajendran K, Halperin JM: More than child’s play: the potential benefits of play-based interventions for young children with ADHD. Expert Rev 2012; 12:1165–1167
146.
Tamm L, Nakonezny PA, Hughes CW: An open trial of a metacognitive executive function training for young children with ADHD. J Atten Disord (Epub ahead of print, May 29, 2012)
147.
Mikami AY, Griggs MS, Lerner MD, Emeh CC, Reuland MM, Jack A, Anthony MR: A randomized trial of a classroom intervention to increase peers’ social inclusion of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol 2013; 81:100–112
148.
Still GF: Some abnormal psychical conditions in children: the Goulstonian lectures. Lancet 1902; 1:1008–1011

Information & Authors

Information

Published In

History

Published in print: Winter 2016
Published online: 14 January 2016

Authors

Details

Philip Shaw, M.B.B.Ch., Ph.D.
Argyris Stringaris, M.D., Ph.D.
Ellen Leibenluft, M.D.

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Citations

Export Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

For more information or tips please see 'Downloading to a citation manager' in the Help menu.

Format
Citation style
Style
Copy to clipboard

View Options

View options

PDF/EPUB

View PDF/EPUB

Get Access

Login options

Already a subscriber? Access your subscription through your login credentials or your institution for full access to this article.

Personal login Institutional Login Open Athens login
Purchase Options

Purchase this article to access the full text.

PPV Articles - Focus

PPV Articles - Focus

Not a subscriber?

Subscribe Now / Learn More

PsychiatryOnline subscription options offer access to the DSM-5-TR® library, books, journals, CME, and patient resources. This all-in-one virtual library provides psychiatrists and mental health professionals with key resources for diagnosis, treatment, research, and professional development.

Need more help? PsychiatryOnline Customer Service may be reached by emailing [email protected] or by calling 800-368-5777 (in the U.S.) or 703-907-7322 (outside the U.S.).

Media

Figures

Other

Tables

Share

Share

Share article link

Share